Adolescent Environmental Tobacco Smoke Exposure Predicts ...

1 downloads 0 Views 113KB Size Report
lems that can disrupt learning and school performance in childhood. Less is known about its ... .0001) ETS, as well as teen smoking, female gender, and lower ...... [26] Baghurst PA, Tong SL, Woodward A, McMichael AJ. Effects of maternal ...
Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

Original article

Adolescent Environmental Tobacco Smoke Exposure Predicts Academic Achievement Test Failure Bradley N. Collins, Ph.D.a,*, E. Paul Wileyto, Ph.D.b, Michael F.G. Murphy, M.D.c, and Marcus R. Munafò, Ph.D.d a

b

Health Behavior Research Clinic, Department of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Tobacco Use Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania c Childhood Cancer Research Group, Department of Paediatrics, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom d Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom Manuscript received December 22, 2006; manuscript accepted April 25, 2007

Abstract

Purpose: Research has linked prenatal tobacco exposure to neurocognitive and behavioral problems that can disrupt learning and school performance in childhood. Less is known about its effects on academic achievement in adolescence when controlling for known confounding factors (e.g., environmental tobacco smoke [ETS]). We hypothesized that prenatal tobacco exposure would decrease the likelihood of passing academic achievement tests taken at 16 and 18 years of age. Methods: This study was a longitudinal analysis of birth cohort data including 6,380 pregnant women and offspring from the 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS). Academic pass/fail performance was measured on British standardized achievement tests (“Ordinary Level” [O-Level] and Advanced Level: [A-Level]). Prenatal tobacco exposure plus controlling variables (ETS, teen offspring smoking and gender, maternal age at pregnancy, maternal smoking before pregnancy, and socioeconomic status) were included in regression models predicting O- and A-Level test failure. Results: Significant predictors of test failure in the O-Level model included exposure to maternal (OR ⫽ 0.71, p ⬍ .0001) and paternal (OR ⫽ 0.70, p ⬍ .0001) ETS, as well as teen smoking, female gender, and lower SES. Prenatal tobacco exposure did not influence failure. Similar factors emerged in the A-Level model except that male gender contributed to likelihood of failure. Prenatal exposure remained nonsignificant. Conclusions: Our model suggests that adolescent exposure to ETS, not prenatal tobacco exposure, predicted failure on both O- and A-Level achievement tests when controlling for other factors known to influence achievement. Although this study has limitations, results bolster growing evidence of academic-related ETS consequences in adolescence. © 2007 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.

Keywords:

Adolescent; Environmental tobacco smoke; ETS; Academic achievement; Longitudinal

Both the United States and United Kingdom produce similar population data related to maternal smoking during pregnancy and children’s exposure to tobacco and its con*Address reprint requests to: Bradley N. Collins, Ph.D., Assistant Professor Departments of Public Health and Pediatrics, Director, Health Behavior Research Clinic, Temple University (265-61), 1701 N. 13th Street Room 161, Philadelphia, PA 19122. E-mail address: [email protected]

stituents. For example, approximately one third of women in their childbearing years are smokers, 10 – 15% of women report smoking during pregnancy [1,2] and up to 60% of children may be exposed to environmental tobacco smoke (ETS) in the home [3]. Smoking during pregnancy is an established risk factor for preterm birth, low birth weight, offspring’s delayed gross and fine motor coordination, and sudden infant death syndrome, accounting for 47% of all neonatal deaths [4 –7] Similarly, children’s ETS exposure

1054-139X/07/$ – see front matter © 2007 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.04.010

364

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

has causal influence on child health problems, many of which influence future adolescent and adult health. A recent cross-sectional, cotinine-verified study of 5,400 children demonstrated health effects across the entire sample of 4- to 16-year-olds, whereby significant effects associated with high cotinine levels included wheezing, impaired lung function, and 6 or more days of absence from school within the last year [8]. Respiratory effects of ETS exposure and the development of respiratory symptoms have been demonstrated in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in 16- to 40-year-olds as well [9 –11]. The consequences of prenatal and childhood exposure to ETS extend beyond health problems. The following sections highlight ETSrelated academic achievement relationships. Prenatal tobacco exposure may directly and indirectly affect academic achievement in childhood as evidenced by the higher risk among exposed versus nonexposed children of displaying cognitive and academic deficits, learning disabilities, impulsivity, auditory processing deficits, and decreased performance on intelligence tests [12–21]. Such evidence has been extended by research on sibling pairs whose mother smoked during one pregnancy but not another [19]. Moreover, prenatal tobacco exposure-academic performance relationships may be mediated by offspring’s tobacco-related behavioral and psychological problems such as attention deficit disorder and conduct disorder [20,22–25]. Few studies have examined the effects of prenatal tobacco exposure on academic achievement or deficits related to achievement beyond childhood. Moreover, studies of prenatal exposure-related neurocognitive consequences [26,27] often demonstrate less definitive results when controlling for confounding psychosocial and environmental factors. Martin et al [28] examined academic achievementrelated effects of prenatal tobacco exposure longitudinally while controlling for confounding variables known to affect offspring development and academic performance. Their results indicated differential effects of prenatal tobacco exposure at infancy, preschool, and school age (age 12 years) [28]. Most relevant to the present paper, Martin et al [28] demonstrated that among offspring at age 12 years, teachers rated prenatal exposed versus non-exposed children as having greater distractibility, negative emotionality, and immaturity as well as less task-oriented behaviors likely to interfere with academic achievement. Direct measures of achievement at age 12 indicated that offspring of smoking mothers had significantly lower end-of-year summary grades than children of nonsmoking mothers. Prenatal tobacco exposure clearly causes many health problems and increases risks of neurocognitive and behavioral problems that can undermine future academic achievement. However, many extant studies do not adequately control for confounding variables including offspring exposure to ETS during childhood [29].

Childhood ETS exposure Evidence of postnatal ETS exposure effects on academic performance is sparse albeit expanding, in comparison to evidence of effects from prenatal exposure. Existing research suggests inverse relationships between recent ETS exposure and young children’s receptive vocabulary skills and reasoning tasks [12]. A review of 17 epidemiological studies that attempted to separate effects of prenatal versus childhood ETS exposure suggested subtle detrimental effects of ETS exposure on children’s neurodevelopment and behavior [15]. ETS exposure caused subtle changes in measures of neurocognitive development and behavior; however, results from many of these studies were difficult to interpret because of the colinearity of prenatal and postnatal exposure [15], as well as potential confounding psychosocial and demographic factors. These methodological issues, together with the relatively small number of ETS studies, make it difficult to determine the degree of influence of prenatal tobacco exposure versus ETS exposure on neurocognitive, behavioral, and academic outcomes. However, recent evidence from Yolton et al [30] examining the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III) conducted between 1988 and 1994 in the U.S. [31] appear to strengthen the call to examine further the prenatal versus concurrent ETS exposure contributions to academic achievement deficits. Using data for 5,365 individuals 6 –16 years old reporting recent tobacco abstinence, their results showed an inverse dose–response relationship between children’s cotinine levels and reading test scores, visuospatial reasoning skills, and math skills—with the negative effects of ETS on reading skills persisting even at low levels of exposure (⬍0.5 ng/mL) [30]. Moreover, a significant ETS-reading skills relationship remained when controlling for confounding effects of prenatal tobacco exposure, birth weight, and NICU admittance among a subsample of 1,266 children [30]. Although their ETS-related data were compelling, their data did not provide comparisons of retrospective, self-reported prenatal tobacco exposure and current ETS exposure effects on academic skills performance. To better understand academic-related tobacco exposure consequences, large, prospective birth cohort studies that include measures of prenatal and concurrent ETS exposure are needed. Purpose Prenatal tobacco exposure–academic performance relationships remain inconclusive when controlling for known confounding factors such as ETS. Moreover, there are no studies that appropriately examine the effects of both prenatal tobacco and ETS exposure on late-adolescent academic achievement. These gaps are important to address, given that both tobacco exposure effects may manifest differently across age ranges. Therefore, while controlling for known confounding factors, we examined the influence of

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

prenatal and adolescent ETS exposure on adolescent offspring achievement test performance. Based on existing evidence, we hypothesized that prenatal exposure to tobacco would significantly increase the likelihood of adolescent academic achievement test failure. Methods Sample and design Information was originally obtained from parents of 17,414 male and female children born in the U.K. March 3– 9, 1958, and from their surviving offspring during follow-up sweeps of survivors at ages 16 and 23 years. Follow-up sweeps reached 10,000-12,000 of the original cohort, with each sweep varying slightly in sample composition due to variable response rate. Data originated from the U.K.’s 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS), an ongoing multidisciplinary investigation of a nationally representative cohort followed from birth into adulthood, with data collection on all study participants attempted at several follow-up “sweeps” by questionnaire, examination, or interview at varying ages between 1958 and 1991. These data are available for secondary analysis from the Data Archive at the University of Essex (http://www.data-archive.ac.uk/). Data obtained for this study were derived from parent and offspring reports when offspring were 16 and 23 years old, respectively. Outcome measure Adolescent offspring academic achievement was measured by unverified, retrospective self-report when offspring were 23 years old (1981). Achievement was classified as pass versus fail, and was determined by performance on nationally standardized achievement tests taken at 16 years of age (“Ordinary Level” [O-Level]) and 18 years old (“Advanced Level” [A-Level]). In the U.S., these tests would be loosely akin to a general high school achievement test (O-Level) and a college entrance exam such as the Scholastic Achievement Test (SAT) (A-Level). Predictor variables Maternal smoking during pregnancy The only data available to determine fetal offspring exposure to tobacco was 1958 maternal report of smoking after the 4th month of pregnancy. Smoking status, determined by unverified maternal self-report at the end of pregnancy, was coded dichotomously (smoker vs. nonsmoker). Offspring ETS exposure at age 16 years Via an unverified maternal self-report in 1974, we included three variables to capture adolescents’ ETS exposure in the home, including daily maternal and/or paternal cigarette smoking (at least one smoker vs. nonsmoker), and daily paternal cigar/pipe smoking (yes vs. no).

365

Controlling variables Maternal smoking before pregnancy with target offspring A maternal self-report from 1958 provided information on maternal smoking (smoker vs. nonsmoker) before pregnancy with target offspring. Mother’s age during pregnancy with target offspring Because of nonproportionality across the age range, we constructed this variable to represent three age categories: ⬍20 years, 21–24 years, and ⬎24 years. Parental socioeconomic classification (SEC) Data on parental SES were derived from self-report of paternal occupation and coded as SEC I-V according to the Registrar General’s Classification of Socioeconomic Status. Cases classified as unemployed or sick (n ⫽ 5) were excluded. The SEC variable used in the analysis was an average value including the observation taken at birth, and the two subsequent time points. Offspring gender Gender was included to account for potential biases that could influence differential test performance between boys and girls. Adolescent smoking status Offspring report of smoking during adolescence was included to control for the influence of smoking on academic performance and to capture effects of latent adolescent psychosocial variables known to influence academic performance. We differentiated between light smoking and heavy smoking (⬍ 10 vs. ⱖ 10 cigarettes per day). Analysis The overall analysis strategy was to model dichotomous, pass versus fail achievement (1, 0). The inclusion criterion was provision of self-report data at both follow-up time points (1974 and 1981). We conducted two logistic regression analyses to analyze test performance on the O- and A-Level examinations, respectively given that success on the O-Level is a prerequisite for taking the A-Level, thereby creating a smaller, subsample of students taking the A-Level examination. All analyses were conducted using Stata software (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX), and results are presented as odds ratios (OR) together with 95% confidence intervals. We used the cluster-correlated robust covariance to adjust standard errors for within-subject correlation. Results Characteristics of offspring and parents Every surviving offspring who provided complete selfreport data at both follow-up time points were included in the study. For outcomes measured both at 16-year (1974) and 23-year (1981) follow-ups, this resulted in a final com-

366

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

Table 1 Parental attribute comparisons between complete and incomplete samples Level

Paternal socioeconomic status at time of birth

Maternal smoking before pregnancy

Maternal smoking late in pregnancy Maternal smoking at 16-year follow-Up

Paternal smoking at 16-year follow-up

Age of mother (y)

I II III Nonmanual III Manual IV V Nonsmoker, ⬍1 daily 1–4 daily 5–14 daily 15–19 daily 20 or more No Yes Does not smoke Less than 1 a day 1–5 per day 6–10 per day 11–20 per day 21 or more Pipe or cigar only Does not smoke Less than 1 a day 1–5 per day 6–10 per day 11–20 per day 21 or more Pipe or cigar only 16–19 20–29 30–39 40–49

plete sample of n ⫽ 6380, representing approximately 41% of the original live births, and between 60% and 72% of the maximum number reached in follow-up sweeps. Table 1 compares the parental attributes of those in the final complete sample with those who were excluded for incomplete data. Table 2 shows offspring demographic and smoking characteristics of the final sample1 it also compares offspring characteristics of the final complete data sample and those excluded from the analyses. O-Level achievement model Table 3 illustrates the O-Level test performance model. This model suggests that adolescents were more likely to 1

Although chi-square tests indicated that attributes differ across the two groups, an examination of the relative frequencies suggested that they are largely uniform across the data groups and that the statistical significance observed was due to the chi-square tests being overpowered as a result of the large sample size. For example, the significant 5.2 point difference in the percentage of mothers smoking late in pregnancy corresponds to an effect size of d ⫽ 0.0007, and the 2.7 point difference in the percentage males at birth corresponds to an effect size of d ⫽ 0.0004.

(%) Incomplete

Complete

4.45 12.31 9.34 50.94 12.04 10.92 57.03 7.64 14.74 14.37 6.22 64.79 35.21 49.4 2.9 5.89 11.84 23.25 6.56 .16 35.31 2.15 4.97 10.31 25.39 12.76 9.11 6.47 59.51 31.11 2.91

4.67 13.99 10.21 50.81 12.25 8.08 62.13 7.75 12.48 12.98 4.65 69.01 30.99 54.49 3.68 5.73 10.7 20.45 4.89 .06 39.97 1.68 3.99 10.21 24.15 10.92 9.08 4.24 63.45 30.27 2.04

␹2(5) ⫽ 44.16 p ⬍ .0001

␹2(4) ⫽ 55.81 p ⬍ .0001

␹2(1) ⫽ 31.98 p ⬍ .0001 ␹2(6) ⫽ 48.56 p ⬍ .0001

␹2(6) ⫽ 32.61 p ⬍ .0001

␹2(3) ⫽ 58.39 p ⬍ .0001

fail the examination if they were exposed to maternal and paternal cigarette ETS, smoked cigarettes themselves (particularly 10 or more cigarettes/week), were female, had a younger mother, and had a lower SES status. Maternal smoking prior to pregnancy and smoking during pregnancy (offspring prenatal exposure) were not significant predictors of academic failure. A-Level achievement model Consistent with the O-Level model, prenatal tobacco exposure did not influence test performance on the A-Level examinations; however, ETS exposure did predict failure. Table 4 shows that adolescents were more likely to fail the A-Levels if they were exposed to maternal and paternal cigarette ETS, smoked cigarettes themselves (particularly ⱖ10 cigarettes/week), were male, had a younger mother, and had a lower social classification (SEC). Discussion This study suggests that recent ETS exposure is a more important factor than prenatal tobacco exposure in predict-

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

367

Table 2 Offspring demographic and smoking characteristics of the final sample Level

Sex

(%)

Male Female Single, divorced, widowed Legally married Professional Intermediate Skilled nonmanual Skilled manual Semi-skilled nonmanual Semi-skilled manual Unskilled manual Nonsmoker At 16-year follow-up Nonsmoker At 23-year follow-up Nonsmoker At 33-year follow-up

Marital status (1981) First job status at 23-year follow-up

Smoking status

Consumption among daily smokers

Cigarettes/day At 23-year follow-up Cigarettes/day At 33-year follow-up

ing adolescent achievement test performance. Consistent in both regression models, exposure to both maternal and paternal cigarette smoke at age 16 years significantly predicted failure. Exposure to paternal cigar/pipe smoke did not influence test performance, perhaps due to the relatively small number of paternal (n ⫽ 974) cigar/pipe smokers compared to maternal (n ⫽ 4,992) and paternal (n ⫽ 5,457) cigarette smokers. Maternal smoking during pregnancy did not significantly contribute to the models of test failure. Table 3 Ordinary-Level (O-Level) examination failure model (n ⫽ 6380) O-Level failure

OR

95% CI

p⫽z

Father’s socioeconomic classification Offspring gender (1 ⫽ female) Mother young ⬍23 years (ref. group) Mother age ⱖ23 years Maternal smoking before pregnancy Maternal smoking late in pregnancy Mother smoked cigarettes (offspring ETS exposure) Father smoked cigarettes (offspring ETS exposure) Father smoked pipe (offspring ETS exposure) Adolescent smoking ⬍1/week (ref.) Adolescent smoking 1–9/week Adolescent smoking ⱖ10/week

.64 1.40 1.00 1.16 .99 .91

.60, .67 1.25, 1.56

.0001 .0001

1.01, 1.33 .95, 1.05 .74, 1.13

.03 .94 .39

.71

.61, .82

.0001

.70

.62, .79

.0001

1.10 1.00 .55 .33

.88, 1.37

.42

.44, .69 .29, .37

.00001 .00001

LR chi (10) ⫽ 1003.87; Prob ⬎ chi ⫽ .00; Log likelihood ⫽ ⫺3599.57; Pseudo R2 ⫽ .12; CI ⫽ confidence interval; ETS ⫽ environmental tobacco smoke; OR ⫽ odds ratio. 2

2

Incomplete

Complete

52.61 47.39 55.42 44.58 2.41 10.34 36.72 25.38 3.46 16.36 5.33 65.15

49.99 50.01 55.41 44.59 3.34 11.22 39.23 24.43 2.92 15.06 3.81 69.38

38.32

43.86

64.16

70.16

Mean (SD) 15.64 (8.88) 16.83 (9.36)

15.34 (8.81) 16.11 (9.61)

x2 (1) ⫽ 11.45 p ⫽ .001 x2 (1) ⫽ .00 p ⬍ 1.00 x2 (6) ⫽ 38.65 p ⬍ .0001

x2 (1) ⫽ 24.27 p ⬍ .0001 x2 (1) ⫽ 27.51 p ⬍ .0001 x2 (1) ⫽ 45.70 p ⬍ .0001 t(5065) ⫽ 1.20 p ⫽ .23 t(3742) ⫽ 2.31 P ⫽ .02

Our results emphasize the importance of the inclusion of confounding variables such as ETS and other factors known to predict academic performance when assessing effects of prenatal tobacco exposure on offspring outcomes such as academic performance. Among other controlling variables in our models, teen smoking was a significant predictor of O- and A-Level performance, showing higher smoking rates increasing the likelihood of failure. SEC predicted failure on both examinations, with slightly greater effect on the A-Level tests. It is likely that both teen smoking and SEC capture latent biopsychosocial comorbidity factors that Table 4 Advanced-Level (A-Level) examination failure model completed by students previously passing the ordinary-Level (O-Level) examination (n ⫽ 4189) A-Level failure

OR

Father’s social class Sex female Mother young ⬍23 years (ref. group) Mother ⱖ23 years Maternal smoking before pregnancy Maternal smoking late in pregnancy Mother smoked (1974) Father smoked (1974) Father smoked pipe (1974) Adolescent smoking ⬍1/week (ref.) Adolescent smoking 1–9/week Adolescent smoking ⱖ10/week

.63 .84 1.00 1.38 1.02 .82 .76 .73 .92 1.00 .75 .29

95% CI

p⫽z

.59, .66 .74, .97

.00001 .01

1.15, 1.67 .95, 1.10 .62, 1.08 .63, .92 .63, .85 .73, 1.16

.001 .58 .16 .005 .00001 .47

.56, 1.00 .23, .36

.05 .00001

LR chi2 (10) ⫽ 597.21; Prob ⬎ chi2 ⫽ .00; Log likelihood ⫽ ⫺2447.62; Pseudo R2 ⫽ .11; CI ⫽ confidence interval; OR ⫽ odds ratio.

368

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

could influence offspring’s ability to pass the O-Levels and their choice to take, as well as their ability to pass, the A-Levels. Mother’s age at pregnancy was another such variable, with younger mothers delivering offspring that were more likely to fail in both models. The effect of mothers’ age could reflect differential parenting care affecting offspring academic performance. Females in our sample were more likely to fail the O-Level tests, and males were more likely to fail the A-Level tests. The direction of this relationship was different than expected in the O-Level model, given that girls typically outperform boys in standardized achievement tests. Perhaps the direction of results with the O-Level tests reflects, in part, the sociocultural climate of 1950 –1970 Britain wherein mid20th Century gender roles and expectations for girls’ career goals may have contributed to girls’ performance on or interest in the O-Levels. Our study demonstrates the predictive effects of adolescent ETS exposure on adolescent achievement test failure. Nonetheless, there are several limitations to the present study. Primary limitations reflect the inherent constraints of the dataset. For example, maternal report of smoking during pregnancy was limited to answering whether she smoked after month 4 of pregnancy. It is impossible to determine from this variable whether offspring were exposed to tobacco in the first trimester, when tobacco-related neurocognitive damage would likely be the greatest, or the degree or dosages of exposure (e.g., number of weeks, etc.) Nonetheless, given that relatively few women in 1950s United Kingdom were likely warned about the dangers of smoking during pregnancy, it is safe to assume that smoking during the second trimester could relate to smoking in the other trimesters, although there is no way to be certain with the data available. We were also limited to using a gross, dichotomous achievement outcome as opposed to a continuous measure, and there were no variables that would enable testing potential mechanisms of the relationship between ETS exposure and academic performance. We were unable to include potential moderator variables, such as maternal psychopathology during pregnancy or postpartum. However, the results from this study can stimulate future, more fine-grained analyses in this area of interest. Future studies also could attempt to disentangle ETS-related neurocognitive consequences from other biopsychosocial and environmental factors that affect academic performance and examine potential interactions of psychosocial and genetic factors in the expression of ETS exposure-related academic problems. Also, our data consisted of unverified self-report. Although a limitation, the likelihood of misreporting could be considered low given that respondents had nothing to gain from misreporting and would have known their reports could be verified by existing test records. With regard to smoking behavior, evidence of the reliability of anonymous surveys of adolescent smoking behavior [32] suggests there

is no specific reason to expect under-reporting of smoking. Likewise, we are not aware of any research that would increase concern of over-reporting of ETS exposure. Our data also lacked biochemical verification of maternal and offspring smoking, a considerable limitation by modern research standards that should be considered when interpreting these results. Also, the dataset did not provide data to examine potential variability in cumulative ETS exposure during childhood. Reported ETS exposure was only available at one time point, age 16 years. However, it is reasonable to assume that offspring were exposed to ETS throughout their childhood given the national smoking rates during the 1950s through 1970s and lack of information about deleterious effects of ETS. Another argument could be made that all sample respondents would have been exposed to ETS on a daily basis throughout the community—as mothers and offspring. Indeed, this sample would have been exposed to a large lifetime dose of ETS in comparison to today (lower smoking rates, greater number of public smoking bans.) Thus ETS could have undermined all respondents’ academic performance. Because it is impossible to determine dosage effects with the data available, we cannot test this assumption. Therefore it is important to consider that direct exposure by parents can amplify ETS consequences that may exist with any exposure. Final limitations include the dataset’s inability to determine how long smoking teens had been smoking before age 16 years and the decision to not include other risk factors such as maternal and offspring other substance use and psychopathology. However, it is well known that smoking is strongly correlated with substance use and psychopathology, and smoking would account for some of this latent variance in our models. Nonetheless it is impossible to disentangle these potential interrelationships and their effects on academic performance. When considering these limitations for future studies, Wakschlag et al [20] provide appropriate guidelines for prospective studies to better enable interpretation of causality and to examine timing and dosage aspects of exposure and its academic consequences. The key challenge is following exposed offspring prenatally through adolescence to identify early versus concurrent versus cumulative exposure-related behavioral and neurocognitive vulnerabilities that may or may not cause adverse behavioral outcomes over time [20]. Despite the limitations stated above, a primary strength of this study is that it generates testable hypotheses for future research aimed to improve our understanding of relationships between ETS and adolescent academic performance. Future research could explore hypotheses within theoretical frameworks that can tease apart reciprocal interrelationships and interactions of the many biological and psychosocial factors that could account for the expression of academic deficits among tobacco exposed offspring beyond

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

what was possible in this study. Biopsychosocial models provide such a framework for complex, multi-determined relationships between exposure and behavior. Within these models, direct and indirect effects of prenatal and childhood exposure to ETS on academic performance can be tested with other predictors, such as psychosocial (e.g., maternal psychopathology, parenting skills, etc.) and biological (e.g., fetal alcohol exposure, adolescent substance abuse, etc.) risk factors and potential mediating variables (e.g., cognitive ability, childhood externalizing disorders, ETS-related illness, etc.). In this study, many of these “other,” etiologically important psychosocial and biological risk factors for academic failure would be considered latent variables in our model—we could not measure or capture them, yet their influence were likely captured because of their inter-relationships among variables that we could capture. Without the opportunity to enter important sources of heterogeneity such as developmental course, exposure dosage, and comorbidity in this study gives rise to opportunity for future researchers in this area who could consider using latent class or mixed models analyses to examine the complex set of multiple predictors and mediators that influence adolescent academic achievement. To guide future research, we speculate that part of the influence of ETS may relate to health-related consequences of exposure. Research has established that ETS exposure increases incidence and severity of a variety of acute and chronic illness in children [33–36] as well as adolescents and adults [9 –11,37]. Increased frequency and severity of illnesses would contribute to school absences and missed academic opportunities. ETS-related illness (e.g., otitis) could contribute to learning skills deficits (e.g., auditory processing) that could undermine academic skills development. Yet another factor that could mediate the ETSacademic performance link could be ETS-related behavioral problems that disrupt academic development and subsequent achievement [19]. Perhaps the teen smoking variables in our models capture variance of potential mediating factors such as conduct problems and other substance abuse that influence academic failure more than the ETS variables account for alone. For example, it is well known that tobacco use and individuals with nicotine dependence have increased risk for alcohol and illicit drug dependence, major depression, anxiety disorders, impulsivity, risk taking, and antisocial personality disorder [38,39]. One feature of our models is that both ETS exposure and teen offspring smoking simultaneously related to academic failure when adjusting for the same confounding factors. These two variables may capture different, albeit overlapping, latent factors that influence academic failure. Bryant et al [40] tested a priori models of the interrelationships between academic achievement, social bonding, school misbehavior and cigarette use among adolescents from grades 8 –12. Their models suggested that

369

school misbehavior, low academic achievement, and cigarette use were interrelated. This study gives rise to many opportunities for future studies to examine differential effects of prenatal tobacco versus ETS exposure on academic achievement within the context of other confounding factors. More current longitudinal studies could ensure appropriate measures of ETS exposure, and could examine potential differential age-related manifestations of tobacco exposure as well as ensure biochemically verified reports of smoking behavior. Policy and treatment implications Our study supports growing evidence that ETS is an environmental toxin that affects academic performance. Evidence herein should further encourage multi-pronged efforts to reduce adolescents’ ETS exposure. This research points to the need to improve our understanding of the influence of ETS on neurocognitive systems, development, and subsequent academic performance—and how ETS exposure consequences might manifest differently at different age ranges during childhood and adolescence. Regarding implications for smoking treatment and ETS-exposure prevention, we emphasize that these data highlight the importance of helping smoking parents create smoke-free homes for their children— outcomes that can be achieved without requiring the immediate parental smoking cessation. Indeed, parental smoking cessation is the ideal outcome in reducing children’s ETS exposure. Nonetheless, health professionals are beginning to advocate for referrals to ETS reduction interventions as a positive alternative to smoking cessation treatment for those parents not willing or ready to consider abstinence-only treatment [41]. Finally, this study suggests the importance of addressing ETS exposure and parental smoking regardless of the child’s age.

References [1] Ananth CV, Kirby RS, Kinzler WL. Divergent trends in maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy: United States 1990 –99. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2005;19:19 –26. [2] CDC. Births: Preliminary data for 2004. Nat Vital Stat Rep 2005; 54:18. [3] Health Consequences of Involuntary Exposure to Tobacco Smoke: A Report of the Surgeon General. In: US Department of Health and Human Services Centers of Disease Control and Prevention Coordinating Center for Health Promotion National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion; 2006. [4] Bibby E, Stewart A. The epidemiology of preterm birth. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 2004;25(Suppl 1):43–7. [5] Floyd RL, Rimer BK, Giovino GA, Mullen PD, Sullivan SE. A review of smoking in pregnancy: Effects on pregnancy outcomes and cessation efforts. Annu Rev Public Health. 1993;14:379 – 411. [6] Magee BD, Hattis D, Kivel NM. Role of smoking in low birth weight. J Reprod Med 2004;49:23–7. [7] Sundell HW. SIDS prevention– good progress, but now we need to focus on avoiding nicotine. Acta Paediatr 2004;93:450 –2.

370

B.N. Collins et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) 363–370

[8] Mannino DM, Caraballo R, Benowitz N, Repace J. Predictors of cotinine levels in US children: Data from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Chest 2001;120:718 –24. [9] Euler GL, Abbey DE, Magie AR, Hodgkin JE. Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease symptom effects of long-term cumulative exposure to ambient levels of total suspended particulates and sulfur dioxide in California Seventh-Day Adventist residents. Arch Environ Health 1987;42:213–22. [10] Jaakkola MS, Jaakkola JJ, Becklake MR, Ernst P. Effect of passive smoking on the development of respiratory symptoms in young adults: An 8-year longitudinal study. J Clin Epidemiol 1996;49: 581– 6. [11] Schenker MB, Samet JM, Speizer FE. Effect of cigarette tar content and smoking habits on respiratory symptoms in women. Am Rev Respir Dis 1982;125:684 –90. [12] Bauman KE, Flewelling RL, LaPrelle J. Parental cigarette smoking and cognitive performance of children. Health Psychol 1991;10:282– 8. [13] Butler NR, Goldstein H. Smoking in pregnancy and subsequent child development. Br Med J 1973;4:573–5. [14] Ernst M, Moolchan ET, Robinson ML. Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:630 – 41. [15] Eskenazi B, Castorina R. Association of prenatal maternal or postnatal child environmental tobacco smoke exposure and neurodevelopmental and behavioral problems in children. Environ Health Perspect 1999;107:991–1000. [16] Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R. Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 13- to 16-year-olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol Teratol 2003;25:427–36. [17] Olds DL, Henderson CR Jr., Tatelbaum R. Prevention of intellectual impairment in children of women who smoke cigarettes during pregnancy. Pediatrics 1994;93:228 –33. [18] Sexton M, Fox NL, Hebel JR. Prenatal exposure to tobacco: II. Effects on cognitive functioning at age three. Int J Epidemiol 1990; 19:72–7. [19] Weitzman M, Byrd RS, Aligne CA, Moss M. The effects of tobacco exposure on children’s behavioral and cognitive functioning: Implications for clinical and public health policy and future research. Neurotoxicol Teratol 2002;24:397– 406. [20] Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Cook E Jr., Benowitz NL, Leventhal BL. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and severe antisocial behavior in offspring: A review. Am J Public Health 2002;92:966 –74. [21] Dunn HG, McBurney AK, Sandraingram, Hunter CM. Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and the child’s subsequent development: II. Neurological and intellectual maturation to the age of 6 1/2 years. Can J Public Health 1977;68:43–50. [22] Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT. Maternal smoking before and after pregnancy: Effects on behavioral outcomes in middle childhood. Pediatrics 1993;92:815–22. [23] Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ, Horwood LJ. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psychiatric adjustment in late adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:721–7. [24] Milberger S, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Chen L, Jones J. Is maternal smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children? Am J Psychiatry 1996;153:1138 – 42.

[25] Silberg JL, Parr T, Neale MC, Rutter M, Angold A, Eaves LJ. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and risk to boys’ conduct disturbance: An examination of the causal hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:130 –5. [26] Baghurst PA, Tong SL, Woodward A, McMichael AJ. Effects of maternal smoking upon neuropsychological development in early childhood: Importance of taking account of social and environmental factors. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 1992;6:403–15. [27] McGee R, Stanton WR. Smoking in pregnancy and child development to age 9 years. J Paediatr Child Health 1994;30:263– 8. [28] Martin RP, Dombrowski SC, Mullis C, Wisenbaker J, Huttunen MO. Smoking during pregnancy: Association with childhood temperament, behavior, and academic performance. J Pediatr Psychol 2006; 31:490 –500. [29] DiFranza JR, Aligne CA, Weitzman M. Prenatal and postnatal environmental tobacco smoke exposure and children’s health. Pediatrics 2004;113(4 Suppl):1007–15. [30] Yolton K, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Lanphear BP, Hornung R. Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents. Environ Health Perspect 2005;113:98 –103. [31] Plan and operation of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988 –94. Series 1: Programs and collection procedures. Vital Health Stat 1 1994:1– 407. [32] Henriksen L, Jackson C. Reliability of children’s self-reported cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 1999;24:271–7. [33] Corbo GM, Fuciarelli F, Foresi A, De Benedetto F. Snoring in children: Association with respiratory symptoms and passive smoking. Br Med J 1989;299:1491– 4. [34] Huang SW, Giannoni C. The risk of adenoid hypertrophy in children with allergic rhinitis. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 2001;8:350 –5. [35] Stahlberg MR, Ruuskanen O, Virolainen E. Risk factors for recurrent otitis media. Pediatr Infect Dis 1986;5:30 –2. [36] Strachan DP, Cook DG. Health effects of passive smoking. 4. Parental smoking, middle ear disease and adenotonsillectomy in children. Thorax 1998;53:50 – 6. [37] Mannino DM, Moorman JE, Kingsley B, Rose D, Repace J. Health effects related to environmental tobacco smoke exposure in children in the United States: Data from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2001;155: 36 – 41. [38] Breslau N. Psychiatric comorbidity of smoking and nicotine dependence. Behav Genet 1995;25:95–101. [39] Dinn WM, Aycicegi A, Harris CL. Cigarette smoking in a student sample: Neurocognitive and clinical correlates. Addict Behav 2004; 29:107–26. [40] Bryant AL, Schulenberg J, Bachman JG, O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Understanding the links among school misbehavior, academic achievement, and cigarette use: A national panel study of adolescents. Prev Sci 2000;1:71– 87. [41] Collins BN, Levin K, Bryant-Stephens T. Exploring pediatricians’ practices and attitudes about ETS and parental smoking: Training issues to promote the reduction of children’s ETS exposure. J Pediatr 2007;150:547–52.