chapter 18

1 downloads 0 Views 658KB Size Report
Nov 22, 2016 - decline, especially during the transitions from primary to secondary ...... [Epub ahead of print] ..... S. Karabenick & T. Urdan (Eds.), The decade.
Intrinsic Motivation

CH A P T ER 18 Interest Theory and Application

JUDITH M. HARACKIEWICZ MAXIMILIAN KNOGLER

While an exact understanding of human motivation continues to evolve, some concepts have proven conducive to our understanding of motivation and the further advancement of motivational models and theories. One of these concepts is intrinsic motivation, which is often related to, and contrasted with, extrinsic motivation. This distinction does not refer to quantitative aspects, such as the amount or intensity of motivation people bring to a task, but to different qualities or kinds of motivation related to why people engage in a certain task or behavior (Ryan & Deci, 2000). The differences relate to different motives, reasons, attitudes, or goals that underlie peoples’ behaviors and actions. On the one hand, extrinsically motivated behaviors are governed by the prospect of some instrumental gain or loss. Individuals who are extrinsically motivated engage with tasks because they expect that their engagement will result in desirable outcomes, such as monetary rewards, high grades, or praise, or an avoidance of negative consequences, such as stress or pain. On the other hand, individuals engage in intrinsically motivated behaviors because they seem to be ends in themselves rather than a means to a separate outcome (Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford,

2014; Sansone & Harackiewicz, 2000). Intrinsic motivation is based on the natural curiosity people possess (White, 1959) and refers to doing something because it is inherently interesting or enjoyable (Deci & Ryan, 1985). For example, people may freely choose to engage in climbing or birding or regularly visit museums and exhibitions, thereby expressing their passion for sports, nature, art, and history. The positive energy associated with intrinsically motivated activities allows people to expand their knowledge, skills, and competencies related to this motivation fairly effortlessly. Therefore, the concept is highly relevant for this volume, as it connects motivation and competence in a synergistic way. Historically, intrinsic motivation emerged as a concept only fairly recently to explain the motivation for activities and behaviors for which the only rewards are perceptions of interest or the experience of enjoyment (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic motivation was therefore used to explain behaviors for which previous frameworks, such as drive theories or behaviorism, could not account. Today, intrinsic motivation is still a versatile and relevant concept that cuts across several theories of motivation and demands the attention of educators striving to facilitate

334

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 334

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

18. Interest 335

high-­quality learning for their students and the development of their competence in sustainable ways. The experience of intrinsically motivated learning can be supported in environments where people can freely explore and pursue already existing interests, or have the opportunity to explore and appreciate new activities and objects. As theory developed, several frameworks came to include concepts of intrinsic motivation. Self-­ determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) argues that intrinsically motivated behaviors relate to the satisfaction of three innate basic psychological needs for the experience of competence, autonomy, and relatedness. For a high level of intrinsic motivation, people must experience satisfaction of these needs and act on their environment to ensure that those needs are met. According to flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 2013), people engage in intrinsically motivating activities because they seek experiences that reflect complete involvement with the activity, together with the accompanying loss of awareness of time and space. And according to interest theory (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Krapp, 2002), people engage in intrinsically motivated behaviors because of personal preferences to interact with a particular content (individual interest) or due to stimulating task characteristics that, on average, many people find to be interesting (situational interest). In this chapter, we focus on interest as a prototypical intrinsic motivational construct. “Interest” is conceptualized as a content-­specific, motivational variable that can inform us about why individuals are motivated to engage and to learn specific subject matter (Hidi, 2000). Thus, calling an individual interested or not interested always requires a description of his or her object of interest. Individuals can be interested in skydiving or in a particular academic topic, but they cannot be generally interested in the same way that they might be considered to be curious (Grossnickle, 2016), open to experience (Goldberg, 1990), or as having a growth mindset (Dweck, 2006). Theories of academic interest have demonstrated the usefulness of the concept first in terms of a theoretical framework for conceptualizing intrinsic motivation and academic motivation, and second, as a framework for the development of educational interventions

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 335

to promote interest in particular topics, as well as applications to strengthen the relation between a person and an object (Mitchell, 1993; Renninger & Hidi, 2016). In this chapter, we first describe the fundamental concepts of interest theory, which offers a unique perspective on intrinsic motivation in terms of a dynamic person–­object relationship that is consequential for how people learn and develop over time. Motivational theories of interest conceptualize interest in terms of a state-like and a traitlike construct, with a developmental framework connecting the two (Krapp, Hidi, & Renninger, 1992). Interest theory not only provides a descriptive framework for how interest develops, but it also describes ways in which interest can be supported to develop in both a short-term and a sustainable long-term manner (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Renninger & Hidi, 2016). In the second part of the chapter we consider intervention research examining how to promote and sustain interest in educational contexts.

THEORIES AND CONCEPTS Interest as a State‑Like Construct Ideally, there are many tasks and activities in our daily lives that we pursue in a state of interest. We may experience interest while reading well-­ written books, while having good conversations, and while tackling intriguing challenges in our jobs. From an interest theory perspective, being intrinsically motivated can be conceived as being in a state of interest while doing something. In this action-­ related sense, interest captures the desire to engage in activities in the moment and refers to a temporary experience of interest while being engaged with a task (Krapp et al., 1992; Renninger & Hidi, 2016). This concept of state interest focuses on the experience of the present moment. It acknowledges the fact that our level of interest is malleable and can change from one moment to the next. In order to understand these changes, researchers explore the complexity of momentary circumstances often conceptualized as “situations.” Therefore, researchers who investigate state interest are typically looking at person-­in-­context experiences of interest and the changes that result

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

336

from short-term engagement with the environment (Ainley, 2006). According to interest research, the state of interest combines positive affective qualities, such as feelings of enjoyment and curiosity, with cognitive qualities of focused attention, as well as perceptions of value and personal importance (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Linnenbrink-­ Garcia et al., 2010). Thus, being in a state of interest means that positive affective reactions and cognitive functioning are intertwined, which makes cognitive engagement and focusing of attention feel relatively effortless. Thus, the state of interest is an ideal state, and one to strive for whenever possible. This is not only because this state of being interested is typically charged with positive feelings and engagement, but also because interest can energize higher levels of performance. Dewey (1913) characterized “interest” as an undivided activity that combines the assessment of personal importance of the activity and positive emotional evaluations of the activity. Accordingly, during interesting activities, there is no conflict between what people think is important for them and what they like to do (Krapp, 2002). Research findings reveal that when this state is activated, learning and attention feel more effortless (Ainley, Hidi, & Berndorff, 2002; Hidi, 2006), that being in a state of interest is positively related to self-­regulation and persistence (Thoman, Smith, & Silvia, 2011; Tulis & Fulmer, 2013), and that interest increases task engagement (Ainley, 2006; Sansone & Thoman, 2005), as well as the use of deep learning strategies (Flowerday, Schraw, & Stevens, 2004). Interest is a phenomenon that emerges from individuals interacting with their environments (Krapp, 2002). The intrinsic quality of interest lies in the positive interaction between a person and a task, which finds its expression in a state of interest and occurs independently of extrinsic outcomes. The intrinsic quality stems from stimulating task characteristics (task-­intrinsic motivation) that facilitate an individual’s motivation to engage in a task for its own sake (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Hidi, 2000), as well as from personal dispositional preferences for the task that the person brings to the situation

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 336

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

(person-­intrinsic motivation). The study of domain-­specific intrinsic motivation therefore covers two broad types of interest: situational and individual interest. In the following section, we highlight situational and individual interest as two different perspectives on the psychological state of interest. The situational interest perspective views state interest as an immediate consequence of the contextual factors present in a situation. These factors or situational cues are assumed to elicit situational interest across individuals. As such, situational interest emerges from the situation and is bound to it. This volatile view of interest implies that every situation has the power not only to support but also to thwart peoples’ state of interest. If situations fail to support people’s interests, then individuals might lose their interest immediately, even if they came into the situation with some interest. Many researchers have highlighted external influences or environmental triggers as prevailing situational elements that influence states of interest (Durik & Harackiewicz, 2007; Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000). Features of the environment that stimulate interest have also been referred to as “collative variables” (Berlyne, 1963, 1970) or “interestingness of the context” (Krapp et al., 1992; Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2010). Empirically, this has resulted in a substantial body of research investigating content, activities, stimuli, or environmental conditions assumed to generate or discourage interest (Bergin, 1999; Cordova & Lepper, 1996). Various contextual variables or task characteristics embedded in texts, classroom situations, and other contexts have been identified as generating and promoting situational interest (Palmer, 2009; Renninger & Hidi, 2011); these may include factors such as novelty, complexity, challenge, or task conditions that support learners’ choice and autonomy, their feelings of competence, and social relatedness (Deci, 1992; Schraw & Lehman, 2001). At the same time, interest theory offers an individual interest perspective on the state of interest. This perspective highlights the influence of individuals’ dispositions and stable preferences for specific content as a reason for being in a state of interest in a particular situation. Here, the immediate experience of

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

18. Interest 337

interest taps into a well-­developed personal preference to enjoy or cherish a particular content or activity consistently across situations and contexts. Individual interest is conceived as a latent disposition that can be activated in the situation (Ainley, 2006; Ainley & Hidi, 2002). Thus, some researchers use the term actualized individual interest to signify that the experience of interest in some situations is primarily elicited by a person’s latent disposition rather than environmental features (Krapp, 2002; Schraw & Lehmann, 2001). For example, a student may be particularly likely to be in a state of interest during a class that is dealing with one of his or her favorite topics. The individual interest perspective encourages researchers to consider how people enter situations, as well as the situational consistencies in momentary experiences and behaviors. When people enter situations with a high level of interest, for example, they might be protected from losing interest if the situation (without the presence of any other kind of support) matches their interests (Linnenbrink-­Garcia, Patall, & Messersmith, 2012; Tsai, Kunter, Lütke, Trautwein, & Ryan, 2008). For example, a student who loves organic chemistry may be able to get through a boring lecture on the topic, whereas other students might lose interest in the same situation. Similarly, other stable personal characteristics can positively influence interest during learning activities, such as higher levels of prior knowledge (Alexander & Jetton, 1996) or mastery goal orientations (Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto, & Elliot, 1997; Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, Linnenbrink-­ Garcia, & Tauer, 2008; Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, & Harackiewicz, 2008; Tanaka & Murayama, 2014). This demonstrates that the experience of interest can be influenced by person-­level factors, and suggests that interest is not simply a product of situational circumstances. So far, research has not provided any evidence for a phenomenological difference between a state of interest that has its origins primarily in individual or dispositional interest and a state of interest that stems from stimulating situational conditions (Schiefele, 2009). To the individual, both states feel the same, and there is no other way to experience interest other than being in a state of

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 337

interest. Current theory therefore assumes that there is only one kind of interest experience, or psychological state of interest, but that this state can originate from two different sources and is therefore associated with different mechanisms (individual vs. situational) (Hidi & Renninger, 2006). In other words, situational interest and individual interest share the same psychological state (Renninger & Hidi, 2016). Thus, if the state of being interested does not feel different as a function of its source, then questions about its origin are difficult to address empirically. Findings have accumulated about person-­ related variables, situation-­related variables, and their mutual relations that influence states of interest (e.g., Durik, Hulleman, & Harackiewicz, 2015; Tanaka & Murayama, 2014; Tsai et al., 2008). Considered together, these studies show that both internal, personal factors and external, environmental factors can shape an individual’s experience of interest in any given moment (Renninger & Hidi, 2011). In a recent study (Knogler, Harackiewicz, Gegenfurtner, & Lewalter, 2015), we used a latent variable approach combined with repeated measures design to disentangle variance in individuals’ states of interest that might be attributed to either individual or situational sources. We asked students to rate their interest several times in response to different instructional situations related to the same topic (e.g., inquiry, presentation of results, discussion, and reflection). These data were analyzed with latent state–trait models that were used to parse out cross-­ situational, stable variance in repeated measures from situation-­specific variance (Geiser & Lockhart, 2012). Results indicated that half of the variance was situation-­ specific and therefore related to particular instructional situations, whereas the other half was consistent or stable across situations. Thus, even though students were dealing with the same topic across a series of lessons, the different instructional arrangements (i.e., the “situations”) had a strong impact on students’ state interest. These findings therefore offer some indication that situation-­ specific variance might be truly situational. Students’ initial individual interest in the topic, as measured before the instruction

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

338

began, was correlated with the stable cross-­ situational part of variance but unrelated to the situation-­ specific parts. Considered together, these findings offer empirical support that the situational interest perspective and the individual interest perspective are not mutually exclusive ways to look at states of interest. Rather, we found evidence for both types of interest across the learning situations. In line with Krapp’s (2002) argument that “any interest has a history,” there is often an actualizing mechanism at work that influences interest at any given moment, and over time stabilizes individuals’ states of interest. At the same time, there are motivational forces in each and every situation that influence individuals’ states of interest irrespective of their previous experiences with the content. Given the magnitude of effects found in this study, both perspectives need to be considered to explain and conceive of interest as a state phenomenon. Moreover, they also need to be considered as representing two different avenues for promoting interest during learning. Interventions can generally focus on harnessing the power of an already existing interest or on harnessing the power of situational cues that can induce situational interest (as discussed in later sections of this chapter). Just like many other experiences, states of interest are embedded in a flowing stream of situations. And while situational and individual perspectives on these states can be separated conceptually, they are interdependent in the ongoing experience of learners. In order to better understand the experience of interest, future research will have to consider theories and methods that examine the dynamic aspects of situations and situation change. In line with a renewed interest and increasing attention to the general study of situations (e.g., Rauthmann & Shermann, 2015), there are more and more forthcoming studies that treat interest as a stateand situation-­ dependent phenomenon by applying repeated and in situ measurement (e.g., Ainley, 2006; Tanaka & Murayama, 2014). Using data-­analytic procedures that allow the study of interest fluctuations at the within-­person level, future research may provide an even richer description of the situational nature of interest.

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 338

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

Interest as a Trait‑Like Concept An individual interest approach focuses on individuals’ enduring preferences for and a predisposition to reengage in particular activities or domains. Individual interest differs from other motivational concepts because it always refers to a particular person–­object relation. In other words, one is always interested in something. The person–­ object theory of interest (Krapp, 2002) conceives of individual interest as an object-­ specific and trait-like variable that varies between persons but is relatively stable across time and across contexts. People differ in their interests, as some are more and some are less interested, for example, in sports, in climbing or birding, or in a particular research topic. If a person holds a particular individual interest, this is usually the outcome of a positive and long-term engagement with this content. Consider, for example, the case of researchers, who often have a long history of reading, writing, discussing, and experimenting with “personal research interests” over the course of months, years, and decades. Since such a deep and repeated engagement is required and longer periods of time are necessary, individuals usually possess a limited number of well-­developed interests but always have the potential for more, as circumstances change (Hofer, 2010). Most of the time, but not always, people are reflectively aware of and identified with their individual interests, and are able specify them. Many researchers, for example, highlight their set of interests usually in “personal research interest sections” or additionally in an “other interests or hobby sections” on their websites or curricula vitae (CVs). Such a reflective awareness puts learners in a position to pursue their interests actively by seeking out the best opportunities for further development (Renninger & Su, 2012). Individual interests are stable, trait-like concepts that do not simply derive from repeated and long-term engagement but are also used to explain people’s choices and activities. If people have the opportunity to decide how to spend their time without any constraints, we would expect them to consistently choose activities related to their individual interests over other activities. In

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

18. Interest 339

several studies with college students (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, & Elliot, 2000; Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, & Elliot, 2002; Harackiewicz et al., 2008), for example, it was found that interest in psychology, developed in introductory psychology courses, predicted subsequent course taking over several years, and students’ choice of academic major (i.e., whether they majored in psychology or not). In this sense, individual interest has the power to consistently influence students’ behaviors, their learning, as well as their momentary motivational states. Individual interest may also have a cyclic, self-­affirming tendency: Initial individual interest can strengthen and deepen subsequent individual interest. For example, individual interest can act as a filter that directs attention toward some subject content that is related to it but not to other types of content. A bird-­watcher might travel to different countries and see unusual birds, deepening his or her interest in birding more generally. This increases the likelihood of further engagement with that content, which in turn further develops and deepens that interest (Renninger, 2000). Entering settings and contexts with initial individual interest can predispose individuals to experience more interest during activities, which can then promote the development of subsequent individual interest in terms of a deepened connection between a person and some topic or subject (Harackiewicz et al., 2008; Linnenbrink-­Garcia et al., 2012). In terms of construct content, the robust person–­ object relationship, which builds the core of the individual interest concept, has been operationalized in several different ways. Here, we present the two most prominent theoretical conceptions in the interest literature: (1) interest as a two-­component construct that includes positive affect and value and (2) interest as a two-­component construct that includes stored value and accumulated knowledge. The first conceptualization, offered by Schiefele (2001, 2009) and Krapp (2002, 2005), identifies interest as a rather stable set of value beliefs with a close combination of affect- and value-­ related components. In other words, persons with a strong individual interest consistently evaluate their interest object as both

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 339

enjoyable and exciting to interact with, and personally significant and therefore as one element of their stored value system. These beliefs and evaluations not only coincide but also are directly related to their interest object and therefore intrinsic in nature. According to Krapp (2002, 2005), affectand value-­related components stem from a dual regulation system, which assumes both cognitive–­rational and implicit–­affective control mechanisms to operate and manifest in stable beliefs. Empirical studies confirmed that these two sets of beliefs tend to be highly correlated and that their interaction supports positive outcomes such as self-­regulation and performance (O’Keefe & Linnenbrink-­Garcia, 2014). Although both components are critical, the model allows individual interests to be more strongly based on either affect-­related or value-­related beliefs (Schiefele, 2009). For example, Frenzel, Dicke, Pekrun, and Goetz (2012) observed a qualitative temporal shift from a more affect-­based notion of interest to a more value-based notion of interest during adolescence. The shift occurred over the course of five school years (grades 5–9) for the students’ interest in mathematics. The authors concluded that, “younger students tend to predominantly associate positive emotional experiences with the phenomenon of being interested, whereas older students appear to become increasingly aware that being interested also involves the desire to learn more and autonomously choose to reengage in the respective domain” (p. 1078). The theoretical model is flexible and allows for some differences in the configuration of the construct content in terms of more value-­related and more affect-­related interests, but this adds psychometric complexity. However, these results suggest that it is critically important to examine the structure of interest through a developmental lens. The second conceptualization of individual interest was developed by Renninger and Hidi (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Renninger, 2000, 2009; Renninger & Hidi, 2016). Like Krapp (2002, 2005) and Schiefele (2001, 2009), they conceive of individual interest as a multifaceted construct. In their conception, however, individual interest combines stored value and accumulated discourse

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

340

knowledge as major components. According to this perspective, people with an individual interest have more stored value and more knowledge accumulated for their domain of interest than for other domains or activities in which they are involved. In particular, Renninger (2000, 2009) emphasized the centrality of high levels of stored domain knowledge as an important quality of individual interest. The “knowledge” component refers to a person’s developing understanding of the procedures and discourse knowledge of particular activities or ideas. The individual who is interested in climbing, for example, develops climbing skills over time and learns more about different places to climb. This interest emerges in relation to the kind of questioning a person undertakes with respect to particular subject content. The driving forces for knowledge accumulation are so-­called “curiosity questions” that are rooted in already existing knowledge and energize people to further explore content and learn about previously unknown aspects of the domain. This in turn supports the continued development of interest. Considered together, these two conceptualizations of individual interest highlight the three critical components of positive affect, stored value, and stored knowledge. All of these components play an important role in the development and maintenance of interest (Hidi & Renninger, 2006) and might therefore be considered in the operationalization of the construct. However, it is difficult to include all three components psychometrically because stored knowledge is not typically assessed with self-­report measures. In order to avoid complications associated with integrating knowledge measures (see Schiefele, 2009), an operationalization of individual interest can include indicators of content-­specific, knowledge-­seeking intentions or behaviors that are concerned with deepening knowledge and adding new ideas to one’s repertoire (Knogler et al., 2015; Krapp & Prenzel, 2011). Interest Development: From Situation to Disposition From very early on, the idea that interest develops, and can be helped to develop, was as important as the positive consequences

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 340

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

that were thought to be associated with the experience of interest (Dewey, 1913). Since then, theorists have frequently stressed that interest is a variable with a strong developmental character (e.g., Krapp, 2002; Renninger & Hidi, 2016). Indeed, interest development has become the major focus of current interest research (Renninger & Su, 2012). Models of interest development typically take a positive view on development and describe both the possibilities of people forming new interests and the process of how an interest grows and deepens over time as a result of ongoing engagement with particular content. At the same time, theorists consider the case that, as frequently observed in everyday life, interest development can come to a halt, or that a particular interest might regress or fall off without adequate support (Hidi & Renninger, 2006). As with any relational variable, the developmental trajectory becomes a question about the extent to which the characteristics of the learning environment fit with the characteristics of the learner (Renninger & Su, 2012). This is true throughout the developmental continuum. Therefore, research on interest development addresses questions about internal and external factors, as well as how their interaction affects the development and deepening of interest. On the one hand, individuals’ characteristics and their particular strengths and needs have been identified as important determinants for interest development. Demographic variables such as age (Frenzel et al., 2012), gender (Häussler & Hoffmann, 2000; Gilmartin, Li, & Aschbacher, 2006), and socioeconomic status (Aschbacher, Li, & Roth, 2010; Harackiewicz, Canning, et al., 2014) have been shown to influence interest development, as well as psychological variables such as prior interest (Durik & Harackiewicz, 2007; Harackiewicz et al., 2008), prior knowledge (Alexander & Jetton, 1996) and self-­concept (Durik, Schechter, Noh, Rozek, & Harackiewicz, 2015; Marsh, Trautwein, Lüdtke, Köller, & Baumert, 2005). On the other hand, it has been shown that, regardless of how well developed a person’s interest may be and however independent learners have become, the interest experience is not exclusively self-­ sustained, but requires an appropriate environment that supports or at

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

18. Interest 341

least allows people to pursue their interests (Renninger & Hidi, 2011). According to Krapp and colleagues (1992), the process of interest development starts with situational interest and a single, situation-­specific person–­object relationship (e.g., hearing about supernovas for the first time). When people move further in their development of interest, this situational frame is blurred, and over time, this connection gains stability and strength, such that dispositional individual interest refers to a person–­object relationship with a high level of stability across situations and contexts (e.g., strong interest in astronomy) (Renninger, 2009). Coinciding with changes in stability, the development from situational interest to dispositional individual interest is also marked by an underlying shift in the locus of control. Whereas a situational interest is primarily caused by factors external to the individual (e.g., a TV show about supernovas), individual interest stems from internal factors. Thus, interest development refers to two fundamental processes: a strengthening of the tendency to reengage content and an increase in the independence from external support. Hidi and Renninger (2006) have framed this process of a continually evolving person–­object relationship in terms of four distinct and sequential phases. In order for interest to develop, it first needs to be elicited by external factors in a given situation. This first phase is referred to as triggered situational interest. If tasks and content are perceived to be meaningful and involving, interest development may enter its second phase of maintained situational interest. If maintained interest endures beyond the particular situation and is associated with the accumulation of knowledge, it may develop into emerging individual interest and thereby enter its third phase (Harackiewicz et al., 2008). Given that knowledge and stored value increase further, learners may eventually enter the fourth phase of well-­developed individual interest. Hidi and Renninger argued that with this development comes a qualitative change from what may be considered primarily an emotion at the initial triggering of situational interest to a greater emphasis on cognitive components in later phases. Moreover, as interest

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 341

deepens across these four phases, individuals develop an increasing metacognitive awareness of their own interest (Renninger & Hidi, 2016). A main contention of the four-phase model of interest development is that interest development is sequential, and that this sequence can be disrupted at any time. This also implies that interest development can go dormant. Whether development continues will depend on not only the person but also on the possibilities and opportunities provided by the environment. Interests that are piqued by situational factors but not supported in subsequent situations may become dormant, and interests can be abandoned at any stage of development if situations do not afford support and continued stimulation. In the following section, we highlight recent research that examines how interest can be promoted in educational contexts.

PROMOTING INTEREST IN EDUCATIONAL CONTEXTS Educators and policymakers have called on motivational researchers to align the agenda of advancing motivational theories with use of research to make a difference in educational contexts (Harackiewicz, Smith, & Priniski, 2016; Kaplan, Katz, & Flum, 2012; Pintrich, 2003; Turner, 2010). To promote this aim, researchers suggested amplifying efforts to investigate motivation in ecological contexts, as well as developing and testing interventions as a means to address critical challenges in student motivation (Harackiewicz, Tibbetts, Canning, & Hyde, 2014; Tibbetts, Harackiewicz, Priniski, & Canning, 2016; Walton, 2014). The major challenge related to interest and intrinsic motivation pertains to the question of how best to support individuals in developing and solidifying their interests in certain areas, as well as how to help learners to become interested and identify with critical subject content, so that they can harness all the potential benefits of interest as they confront challenging courses. Interest as an energizer of task-­ related behavior is relevant in almost every teaching and learning context (Schunk et al., 2010) because students become more engaged

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

342

and learn more when they are interested in the topic. Yet research may be particularly needed in academic domains that many students do not find interesting or in which they typically lose interest as they progress through formal education. For example, there is considerable evidence documenting a decline of students’ academic interest in middle school and high school, particularly in science education and science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM) subjects (e.g., Eccles et al., 1993; Krapp & Prenzel, 2011; Renninger, Nieswandt, & Hidi, 2015). Theorists have highlighted general developmental trends, such as age-­ related changes, as explanations for this decline, especially during the transitions from primary to secondary levels of education. Moreover, concerns have been voiced about the way that science, technology, and mathematics are taught in school (Tröbst, Kleickmann, Lange-­ Schubert, Rothkopf, & Möller, 2016). Instructional practices often seem to fail to actively engage students (Seidel & Prenzel, 2006). Most critically, there seems to be a large gap between what schools offer and what students value, prefer, and are interested in (Brophy, 2008; Potvin & Hasni, 2014). Thus, there is a great potential for changes in instructional practices based on insights from interest research to help counteract these downward trends. In the following sections we consider three general avenues for intervention, all of which are guided by interest theory and target motivational processes and, in turn, educational outcomes. As highlighted earlier, effective support for interest development may also depend on a learner’s phase of interest and variability in other learner characteristics. From an interest theory perspective that emphasizes the match between personal preferences and opportunities provided by the environment, we suggest two general and complementary avenues for intervention (see also Pintrich, 2003): 1. Build on existing individual interest: Provide content material and tasks designed to facilitate the connection of academic content to be learned with already existing interests. 2. Generate situational interest: Provide stimulating tasks, activities, and

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 342

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

materials that use universal structural features (i.e., problems, challenges) in order to trigger and maintain situational interest for all students. Build on Existing Individual Interest: Personalized Instruction The individual interest approach to cultivating interest emphasizes students’ individual preferences as a basis for frequent reengagement. To cultivate the development of interest, this approach promotes building on individual learner characteristics, especially the current interests of the student population. Researchers seek to capitalize on the active role of individual interest in the coregulation of person-­ in-­ context experiences of interest by increasing the fit between content and learners’ individual interests. Of course, efforts in this direction would not seem worthwhile or even necessary if curricula and the content of lessons were already largely aligned with students’ interests. However, it has frequently been pointed out that a core problem with today’s schools and curricula is that academic content is not often a good fit with students’ individual interests (Baram-­ Tsabari, 2015; Baumert & Köller, 1998; Harackiewicz et al., 2016). Indeed, many students pursue their most cherished individual interests outside of school (Bergin, 1999; Hofer, 2010). To build learning environments around existing interests could represent an easy fix for educators, as connections to content do not have to be created from scratch, and the positive effects of individual interest on motivation and performance have been amply demonstrated (Renninger & Hidi, 2011). However, this approach may not be practical for instructors of large classes given the unpredictability and heterogeneity of individual interests among diverse students. Indeed, researchers and practitioners alike have noted that it seems rather challenging and time consuming to cater simultaneously to the personal interests of a heterogeneous group of students (e.g., in a classroom), if students differ significantly in terms of their interests and motivational characteristics for various school tasks (Harackiewicz et al., 2016; Hidi, 2000). Furthermore, curricula are standardized

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

18. Interest 343

and formally restricted, and often provide narrow guidelines regarding content to be studied, which might not support adaptation to students’ interests. However, thanks to the recent shift from input- to output-­driven education, which now provides competence-­ based learning goals instead of precise content for input, teachers are granted more flexibility as to what specific content to choose for competence-­based instruction (Organization for Economic Co-­ operation and Development [OECD], 2007). For example, in current science curricula, the competence of creating a scientifically sound argument is most relevant (Osborne, 2010). Such a competence-­based goal, however, does not overly determine particular content. Thus, teachers are free to choose science topics and instructional strategies that can be more aligned with students’ interests and their everyday life. Moreover, this increasing flexibility also comes at a time when advanced technology systems and learning technologies can provide feasible and scalable solutions for tailoring instruction to learners’ needs and interests (Collins & Halverson, 2009). A possible way to facilitate connections between learners and content that is based on individual interest is use of adaptive approaches to instruction such as context personalization. The practice of context personalization refers to matching instructional tasks or educational content with characters, objects, and themes of students’ out-of-­ school interests (Cordova & Lepper, 1996; Høgheim & Reber, 2015; Walkington & Bernacki, 2014). For example, in a physics class, a learner interested in extreme sports might be given a task that involves parachutes and skydiving to learn about the concept of gravity and air resistance (see Palmer, 2009). The same learner could be given reading assignments based on texts related to extreme sports in English or foreign language classes to extend his or her vocabulary and practice communication skills. Thus, even though there are content constraints about what students are expected to learn, there is flexibility in terms of the choice of context in which this content is embedded. Choosing contexts that relates to students’ interests connects new content and tasks to learner’s preexisting individual

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 343

interests. The positive effects associated with individual interest for learning are hypothesized to transfer onto new content and to foster learners’ experience of interest and, in turn, performance. Evidence from experimental research suggests that context personalization strategies are effective in fostering interest, effort, and performance. In an earlier study (Cordova & Lepper, 1996) with elementary students, individualized information related to students’ backgrounds and interests was inserted in a computer game using arithmetic. Compared to students in nonpersonalized conditions, this led to higher gains in students’ intrinsic motivation, involvement, and learning. A recent review of studies on context personalization (see Walkington & Bernacki, 2014) confirmed these early findings. Studies in middle and high school mathematics indicated that learners adopt more positive attitudes toward personalized rather than generic material. Students displayed more effort and continued to perform better on personalized tasks compared to a control group, even after personalization had been removed (Walkington, 2013). These positive effects were most pronounced in students struggling with mathematics (Walkington, 2013) and among learners with low individual interest (Høgheim & Reber, 2015; Renninger, Ewen, & Lasher, 2002). In line with Durik and Harackiewicz (2007), these studies suggest that context personalization interventions could be particularly useful in supporting less engaged learners who begin a task with lower levels of interest. Studies also suggested that the provision of task choice could further enhance these positive effects, possibly through further increasing the match between learners’ interests and their interest-­related choices in the learning environment (Cordova & Lepper, 1996; Høgheim & Reber, 2015; Palmer, 2009; Patall, 2013). Theoretical mechanisms used to explain these findings are anchored in interest theory. In line with other findings that indicate individual interest in academic content can act as a resource for learning (e.g., Knogler et al., 2015; Tsai et al., 2008), the success of these context manipulations is attributed to the potential of learners’ individual interest to influence momentary instructional

11/22/2016 4:01:21 PM

344

experiences and, in turn, learning outcomes. More specifically, context personalization can support learner motivation, as well as further knowledge acquisition through mechanisms that build on positive affect, perceived value, and accumulated knowledge as the three core components of individual interest. First, tasks and material that connect to a learner’s individual interest are more likely to elicit an immediate positive affective reaction, which may or may not translate into more maintained states of interest. Second, existing amounts of stored value can enhance perceptions of value for the task, which have been shown both to trigger and maintain student’s interest (Hulleman, Godes, Hendricks & Harackiewicz, 2010). Third, learners interested in a subject area such as extreme sports or astronomy are likely to have accumulated some prior knowledge that can act as a catalyst for further knowledge development in this content domain. In conclusion, all three of these mechanisms operating in tandem may prove to be a powerful combination to ground new content effectively in existing affective and cognitive structures, so that they become easier to identify with and to grasp. As only a few studies have addressed these issues so far, the field of context personalization awaits further research that also considers different kinds of interventions. To provide better orientation and to foster systematic research in this area, Walkington and Bernacki (2014) have recently classified context personalization interventions along three dimensions: depth, grain size, and ownership. Depth refers to the quality of the connections to learners’ existing interests established by the intervention. Here, interventions range from simple insertions of surface-­level information about students’ interests (e.g., one’s favorite movie) to very elaborate contextualized tasks that are deeply embedded in students’ interests. Grain size refers to the size of the reference group and differentiates between tasks that are tailored to the interest of an individual learner or to certain groups of learners, such as a particular school class or a certain age group. Ownership addresses the fact that different people can personalize context and therefore own the process. Although personalization might typically

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 344

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

be considered to be the territory of teachers and curriculum designers, learners have also been successfully encouraged to personalize context for themselves by reflecting on content and its relevance to their own lives (Harackiewicz, Canning, Tibbetts, Priniski, & Hyde, 2016; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009; Hulleman et al., 2010; Yeager et al., 2014). Moreover, research suggests that the self-­generation of value statements is more powerful than learning about value connections from other individuals (Canning & Harackiewicz, 2015), supporting the ownership idea. We discuss these utility–­value interventions below. Further research on personalization strategies will need to clarify which combinations of these criteria make the most effective interventions that harness the potential of personalization in fostering important learning outcomes. The easy access to modern computer technology, such as intelligent tutoring systems, offers many, perfectly scalable ways both to assess students’ individual interests and sophisticated methodologies for personalizing instruction. Thus, digital technology significantly lowers the implementation threshold for effectively personalizing interventions, and this can support them in becoming a standard feature of interest-­based STEM education. Generating Situational Interest Utility‑Value Interventions Keeping students interested in their courses is crucial to their academic success. One way to develop interest in activities is to help students find meaning and value in those activities (Harackiewicz & Hulleman, 2010; Harackiewicz et al., 2016), and one type of task value that has proven to be a powerful predictor of interest, effort, and performance is utility value. People find utility value in a task if they believe it is useful and relevant beyond the immediate situation, for other tasks or aspects of their life (e.g., “This material will be important when I shop for healthy food”). Recent experimental research indicates that it is possible to promote perceived utility value with simple interventions that ask students to write about the relevance of course topics to their

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

18. Interest 345

own lives or to the life of a family member or close friend. The utility-­ value intervention (UVI) is based in expectancy–­ value and interest theory. According to Eccles’s expectancy–­ value theory, a person chooses to take on a challenging task—such as persisting in a college biology course or choosing to major in biology—­if the person (1) values the task, and (2) expects that he or she can succeed at the task (based on self-­ beliefs). Beliefs about the self and beliefs about the value of the task are both critically important in predicting interest, course choices, persistence, and choice of a major. In Eccles’s model, task value has several components, including intrinsic value (the enjoyment an individual experiences from performing a task), attainment value (the personal importance of doing well on a task), and utility value (how useful or relevant the task is in terms of the individual’s future plans). Intrinsic value is, of course, closely aligned with situational interest, and there are many interesting overlaps between expectancy–­value and interest theories. The UVI focuses on utility value, however, because it is the task value most amenable to external influence and intervention (Harackiewicz & Hulleman, 2010). In other words, educators may be able to influence students’ perceptions of utility value (UV) with simple interventions, and these perceptions of utility may in turn promote interest development. Extensive experimental and longitudinal survey studies have documented the importance of both expectancy (e.g., confidence) and value-­ related beliefs (perceptions of usefulness and personal relevance). The perceived value of any academic course is influenced by how closely it relates to the student’s identity and both short- and longterm goals (Eccles, 2009). When a student says, “I can do science, but I don’t want to,” such a choice likely reflects a relatively low perceived value of science. When students do perceive value in course topics, however, they develop greater interest in the course, work harder, perform better, persist longer, and are more likely to take additional courses and complete their degree programs (Harackiewicz et al., 2008; Hulleman et al., 2008; Wigfield, 1994). Educators can influence students’ perceptions of UV in science

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 345

courses using writing activities focused on course content. The UVI works by changing how students think about academic topics. On their own and in their own terms, students generate connections between course topics and their lives—­helping them appreciate the value of their coursework and promoting a deeper level of engagement. Thus, the externally administered UVI may help students relate course material to their own interests. As such, the UVI represents a combination of the two approaches to promoting interest: It may spark situational interest in a topic, and it may help students connect that topic to their own interests, which can build on individual interest. Laboratory studies have demonstrated that self-­generated UV information (as produced by the UVI) is much more powerful than externally provided UV information (e.g., as might be produced when teachers tell students that material is important and relevant) in promoting interest and performance (Canning & Harackiewicz, 2015; Durik et al., 2015). The key is having students actively work to find the value for themselves. The efficacy of this approach for promoting perceived UV, interest, and performance has been demonstrated in ninthgrade science and undergraduate introductory psychology, with the strongest benefits for students with low confidence or lower levels of performance (Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009; Hulleman et al., 2010). More recently, Harackiewicz, Canning, Tibbetts, Priniski, and Hyde (2016) documented the potential of the UVI to close achievement gaps for first-­ generation and underrepresented minority students in college biology courses. In addition, Harackiewicz, Rozek, Hulleman, and Hyde (2012) found that a UVI that targeted the parents of high school students led the students to take, on average, an extra semester of math or science in their last 2 years of high school. According to interest theory, being interested in an activity motivates us to pursue activities and careers. Interest may be triggered by UVIs, then develop further as the individual experiences positive feelings and comes to value an activity. By integrating expectancy–­ value and interest theories, we propose two ways that UV can influence interest, motivation, and persistence

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

346

in academic contexts. First, perceiving UV in a course can directly influence interest and subsequent course enrollment choices because of the importance of these courses for future goals. Second, perceiving UV in courses can influence subsequent course choices and career decisions through the process of interest development; that is, perceiving value in courses can promote deeper interest in the topic, and interest may be the more proximal motivator of career decisions. Thus, interest may be a pathway through which UVIs influence motivation and performance (Harackiewicz et al., 2016). Problem‑Based Instruction A situational approach to cultivating interest views interest as a situated and malleable construct that offers a great potential for change and influence in features of the environment. In educational settings, this view highlights the importance of creating a stimulating learning environment in which students are supported in connecting to content, especially when the content is not related to their preferences and existing interests. The situational approach acknowledges that educators do not have influence over students’ incoming individual interest, yet they do have influence over students’ situational interest as a short-term response to the learning environment they create, for example, during a particular lesson. Furthermore, if continued situational support is provided, initial situational interest may develop beyond situational confines and support long-term interest development in a domain (Chen et al., 2016). Situational interest and its antecedents have been extensively studied in the context of text comprehension, which has demonstrated, among other things, that readers are interested in texts that include surprising, novel, or unusual elements and text features such as personal relevance, coherence, and vividness (Ainley et al., 2002; Schraw & Lehmann, 2001). Following Mitchell’s (1993) seminal study in the mathematics classroom, the focus of this research has broadened recently, and more studies are forthcoming that analyze activities and tasks that generate interest in any learning environment for many individuals (see Renninger &

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 346

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES

Hidi, 2011, for a review). Researchers have investigated various instructional practices and their potential role in fostering interest. For the context of STEM education, a recent review (Potvin & Hasni, 2014) identified problem- and inquiry-­based approaches to instruction as very effective in fostering interest and learning. Using interest theory as a framework for modeling motivational dynamics, researchers have used problem-­ based learning environments to analyze the promotion and maintenance of interest (Belland, Kim, & Hannafin, 2013; Palmer, 2009; Rotgans & Schmidt, 2011, 2014; Schmidt, Rotgans, & Yew, 2011; Wijnia, Loyens, & Derous, 2011; Wijnia, Loyens, Derous, & Schmidt, 2014). Problem-­based learning has been defined as “an instructional method that initiates students’ learning by creating a need to solve an authentic problem” (Hung, Jonassen, & Liu, 2007, p. 486). From an interest theory perspective, problems are a means to stimulate curiosity questions that in later phases of interest development are activated from within the person and represent a core mechanism for extending and solidifying his or her interests (Renninger & Su, 2012). Compared to the previously discussed individual interest approach, which taps into individual interest and an associated fund of knowledge as a resource, a problem-­based approach signals to learners that they lack some critical knowledge. This can be an effective trigger for situational interest and stimulate initial engagement with a certain task or domain (Berlyne, 1970; Rotgans & Schmidt, 2014). Since there is no immediate answer for many problems, learners are required to figure out what is unknown to them or to reorganize what they have understood to date. In this sense, situational interest can play an important role in energizing the acquisition of knowledge (Rotgans & Schmidt, 2011). Recently, Rotgans and Schmidt (2014) suggested that learning materials are effective at eliciting situational interest in all learners when they confront learners with an intriguing problem. In several studies, they used history problems and asked secondary students from Singapore why the Japanese were able to conquer the island during the World War II, despite the fact that they were

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

18. Interest 347

highly outnumbered by the Allied Forces. This problem initially triggered students’ situational interest, and their mean levels of interest were significantly higher after problem presentation than before. Students were then provided reading material that contained relevant information to resolve the problem. After reading the text, students’ situational interest decreased again. This pattern was only found if students did not know the solution beforehand and if they were aware of their deficit. Rotgans and Schmidt explained their results with a knowledge deprivation mechanism that construes situational interest as arising from a perceived gap between what students know and what they need or want to know (Schmidt et al., 2011). Initially, problems can create this gap and in turn stimulate situational interest. Subsequently, as students gain relevant knowledge while working to solve the problem, they close the gap, which again reduces their levels of situational interest (Rotgans & Schmidt, 2011, 2014). According to this research, problem-­ based learning environments appear to be a reliable way to generate situational interest with the presentation of challenging problems. However, the findings also suggest that problems may be a rather temporary stimulus, not necessarily leading to repeated engagement, as students’ interest steadily decreases once they start working on and solving the problem. Moreover, these findings may not generalize across all types of problems. In previous research on problem-­ based learning, learning outcomes also depended on the type of problem used. In their meta-­ analysis, Walker and Leary (2009) found complex and ill-­ structured problems to be more effective with regard to student learning than well-­ structured problems. As Rotgans and Schmidt had focused their investigations on problems belonging to the latter category (Jonassen, 2011), Knogler, Gröschner, and Lewalter (2016) tested a complex problem and its capacity to foster students’ situational interest. In line with the knowledge deprivation hypothesis, they assumed complex problems to be more effective in stimulating situational interest. They argued that even though learners may gain relevant knowledge while investigating a complex problem (e.g., climate change), the

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 347

problem cannot be solved as straightforwardly as simple problems; instead, complex problems are evolving in nature and gradually reveal additional layers of complexity. These create newly emerging knowledge gaps as learners acquire a deeper understanding while they continue to investigate und develop solutions (Jonassen, 2011). In their study, Knogler and colleagues (2016) presented to secondary students a problem scenario in which they had to negotiate a solution for the energy supply of a rural district that wanted to shift from nuclear power to renewable sources. The students were then engaged in collaborative problem solving over the course of 15 lessons, exploring and discussing different solutions and their limitations. Repeated measures of situational interest indicated a developmental pattern whereby situational interest was stimulated not just once but repeatedly. In post hoc interviews, students frequently referred to the experience of novelty and the ability to expand their knowledge in the face of novel information as subjective cause for higher levels of situational interest during complex problem solving (see also Palmer, 2009). This confirmed the assumption that a more complex problem structure holds more potential for the continuing perception of knowledge gaps or opportunities to learn compared to well-­structured problems with a single gap. In addition to knowledge-­based mechanisms, rich and complex problem-­ based learning environments also feature a rich array of other contextual stimuli, such as perceived autonomy or social relatedness supportive of situational interest (Krapp, 2005). Thus, problem-­based learning environments may be particularly well suited to support situational interest as they offer challenging problems and an engaging set of learning activities. Additional research can help to further unleash this potential and identify effective problem structures or scaffolding strategies that optimally support learners in confronting these problems (Belland et al., 2013). This line of research is relevant for STEM education because science offers many intriguing and complex problems. To leverage their potential for a more interest-­based STEM education, teachers and curriculum designers would need to create more learning

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

348

environments based on problems. Such an effort would also be in line with recent reforms that promote inquiry-­based teaching and problem-­ based learning together with crosscutting themes and core ideas for science education.

CONCLUSION A careful consideration of the nature of interest, whether conceptualized as an individual-­difference variable, a situational process, a developmental trajectory, an educational outcome, or, as we have argued here, all of these combined, affords insight into important motivational processes. The theoretical and empirical progress to date has yielded several promising directions for intervention in educational contexts. By analyzing the malleable and more stable aspects of interest, we can design interventions that spark the development of new interests or support the further development of existing interests that can shape students’ academic trajectories. REFERENCES Ainley, M. (2006). Connecting with learning: Motivation, affect and cognition in interest processes. Educational Psychological Review, 18, 391–405. Ainley, M., & Hidi, S. (2002). Dynamic measures for studying interest and learning. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: New directions in measures and methods (Vol. 12, pp. 43–76) Amsterdam, The Netherlands: JAI Press. Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest, learning, and the psychological processes that mediate their relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 545–561. Alexander, P. A., & Jetton, T. L. (1996). The role of importance and interest in the processing of text. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 89–121. Aschbacher, P. R., Li, E., & Roth, E. J. (2010). Is science me?: High school students’ identities, participation and aspirations in science, engineering, and medicine. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47, 564–582. Baram-­Tsabari, A. (2015). Promoting information seeking and questioning in science. In K.

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 348

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES A. Renninger, M. Nieswandt, & S. Hidi (Eds.), Interest in mathematics and science learning (pp. 135–152). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Baumert, J., & Köller, O. (1998). Interest research in secondary level I: An overview. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, K. A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and learning: Proceedings of the Seeon Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 241–256). Kiel, Germany: IPN. Belland, B. R., Kim, C., & Hannafin, M. J. (2013). A framework for designing scaffolds that improve motivation and cognition. Educational Psychologist, 48, 243–270. Bergin, D. A. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational Psychologist, 34, 87–98. Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 279–286. Brophy, J. (2008). Developing students’ appreciation for what is taught in school. Educational Psychologist, 43, 132–141. Canning, E. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). Teach it, don’t preach it: The differential effects of directly-­ communicated and self-­ generated utility value information. Motivation Science, 1, 47–71. Cerasoli, C. P., Nicklin, J. M., & Ford, M. T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-­analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 980–1008. Chen, J. A., Tutwiler, M. S., Metcalf, S. J., Kamarainen, A., Grotzer, T., & Dede, C. (2016). A multi-user virtual environment to support students’ self-­efficacy and interest in science: A latent growth model analysis. Learning and Instruction, 41, 11–22. Collins, A., & Halverson, R. (2009). Rethinking education in the age of technology: The digital revolution and schooling in America. New York: Teachers College Press. Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 715–730. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2013). Flow: The psychology of happiness. New York: Random House. Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-­ determination in human behavior. New York: Pantheon. Deci, E. L. (1992). The relation of interest to the motivation of behavior: A self-­determination theory perspective. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 43–71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

18. Interest 349 Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. New York: Houghton Mifflin. Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How individual interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 597–610. Durik, A. M., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). One size fits some: Instructional enhancements to promote interest don’t work the same for everyone. In K. A. Renninger, M. Nieswandt, & S. Hidi (Eds.), Interest in mathematics and science learning (pp. 49–62). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Durik, A. M., Shechter, O., Noh, M. S., Rozek, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). What if I can’t?: Perceived competence as a moderator of the effects of utility value information on situational interest and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 104–118. Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House. Eccles, J. S. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life?: Personal and collective identities as motivators of action. Educational Psychologist, 44, 78–89. Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-­environment fit on young adolecents’ experiences in schools and families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101. Flowerday, T., Schraw, G., & Stevens, J. (2004). The role of choice and interest in reader engagement. Journal of Experimental Education, 72, 93–114. Frenzel, A. C., Dicke, A. L., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2012). Beyond quantitative decline: Conceptual shifts in adolescents’ development of interest in mathematics. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1069–1082. Geiser, C., & Lockhart, G. (2012). A comparison of four approaches to account for method effects in latent state–trait analyses. Psychological Methods, 17, 255–283. Gilmartin, S. K., Li, E., & Aschbacher, P. (2006). The relationship between interest in physical science/engineering, science class experiences, and family contexts: Variations by gender and race/ethnicity among secondary students. Journal of Women and Minorities in Science and Engineering, 12, 179–207. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229. Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Disentangling

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 349

curiosity: Dimensionality, definitions, and distinctions from interest in educational contexts. Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 23–60. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., & Elliot, A. J. (1997). Predictors and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom: Maintaining interest and making the grade. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1284–1295. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Shortterm and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and performance over time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316–330. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575. Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Giffen, C. J., Blair, S. S., Rouse, D. I., et al. (2014). Closing the social class achievement gap for first-­generation students in undergraduate biology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 375–389. Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Priniski, S. J., & Hyde, J. S. (2016). Closing achievement gaps with a utility-­value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. [Epub ahead of print] Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink-­Garcia, L., & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement goals in the development of interest: Reciprocal relations between achievement goals, interest, and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 105–122. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Hulleman, C. S. (2010). The importance of interest: The role of achievement goals and task values in promoting the development of interest. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 42–52. Harackiewicz, J. M., Rozek, C. S., Hulleman, C. S., & Hyde, J. S. (2012). Helping parents to motivate adolescents in mathematics and science: An experimental test of a utility-­value intervention. Psychological Science, 43, 899– 906. Harackiewicz, J. M., Smith, J. L., & Priniski, S. J. (2016). Interest matters: The importance of promoting interest in education. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 220–227.

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

350 Harackiewicz, J. M., Tibbetts, Y., Canning, E. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2014). Harnessing values to promote motivation in education. In S. Karabenick & T. Urden (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 18. Motivational interventions (pp. 71–105). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group. Häussler, P., & Hoffmann, L. (2000). A curricular frame for physics education: Development, comparison with students’ interests, and impact on students’ achievement and self-­ concept. Science Education, 84, 689–705. Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher’s perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 309–339). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Hidi, S. (2006). Interest: A unique motivational variable. Educational Research Review,1(2), 69–82. Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 79, 151–179. Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The fourphase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. Hofer, M. (2010). Adolescents’ development of individual interests: A product of multiple goal regulation? Educational Psychologist, 45, 149–166. Høgheim, S., & Reber, R. (2015). Supporting interest of middle school students in mathematics through context personalization and example choice. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 42, 17–25. Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement goals, and interest: An integrative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 398–416. Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and performance with a utility value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 880–895. Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high school science classes. Science, 326, 1410– 1412. Hung, W., Jonassen, D. H., & Liu, R. (2007). Problem-­based learning. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. V. Merrienboer, & M. P. Driscoll (Eds.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (3rd ed., pp. 485–506). New York: Routledge. Jonassen, D. H. (2011). Learning to solve

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 350

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES problems: A handbook for designing problem-­ solving learning environments. New York: Routledge. Kaplan, A., Katz, I., & Flum, H. (2012). Motivation theory in educational practice: Knowledge claims, challenges, and future directions. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, T. Urdan, S. Graham, J. Royer, & M. Zeidnerm (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook: Vol 2. Individual differences and cultural and contextual factors (pp. 165–194). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Knogler, M., Gröschner, A., & Lewalter, D. (2016, August). What makes and keeps complex problem-­solving interesting? Paper presented at the International Conference on Motivation (ICM), Thessaloniki, Greece. Knogler, M., Harackiewicz, J. M., Gegenfurtner, A., & Lewalter, D. (2015). How situational is situational interest?: Investigating the longitudinal structure of situational interest. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 43, 39–50. Krapp, A. (2002). Structural and dynamic aspects of interest development: Theoretical considerations from an ontogenetic perspective. Learning and Instruction, 13, 383–409. Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest and intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning and Instruction, 15, 381–395. Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest, learning, and development. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Krapp, A., & Prenzel, M. (2011). Research on interest in science: Theories, methods, and findings. International Journal of Science Education, 33, 27–50. Linnenbrink-­Garcia, L., Durik, A. M., Conley, A. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Karabenick, S. A., et al. (2010). Measuring situational interest in academic domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70, 647– 671. Linnenbrink-­Garcia, L., Patall, E. A., & Messersmith, E. E. (2012). Antecedents and consequences of situational interest. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 591–614. Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Köller, O., & Baumert, J. (2005). Academic self-­ concept, interest, grades, and standardized test scores: Reciprocal effects models of causal ordering. Child Development, 76, 397–416. Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted structure in the secondary school mathematics classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 424–436.

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

18. Interest 351 O’Keefe, P. A., & Linnenbrink-­Garcia, L. (2014). The role of interest in optimizing performance and self-­regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 70–78. Organisation for Economic Co-­ operation & Development. (2007). PISA 2006: Science competencies for tomorrow’s world. Paris: Author. Osborne, J. (2010). Arguing to learn in science: The role of collaborative, critical discourse. Science, 328, 463–466. Palmer, D. H. (2009). Student interest generated during an inquiry skills lesson. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 147–165. Patall, E. A. (2013). Constructing motivation through choice, interest, and interestingness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 522– 534. Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation inlearning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 667–686. Potvin, P., & Hasni, A. (2014). Interest, motivation and attitude towards science and technology at K–12 levels: A systematic review of 12 years of educational research. Studies in Science Education, 50, 85–129. Rauthmann, J. F., & Sherman, R. A. (2015). Situation change: Stability and change of situation variables between and within persons. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1938. Renninger, K. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 373–404). New York: Academic Press. Renninger, K. A. (2009). Interest and identity development in instruction: An inductive model. Educational Psychologist, 44, 105– 118. Renninger, K. A., Ewen, L., & Lasher, A. K. (2002). Individual interest as context in expository text and mathematical word problems. Learning and Instruction, 12, 467–490. Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2011). Revisiting the conceptualization, measurement, and generation of interest. Educational Psychologist, 46, 168–184. Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2016). The power of interest for motivation and engagement. New York: Routledge. Renninger, K. A., Nieswandt, M., & Hidi, S. (Eds.). (2015). Interest in mathematics and science learning. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Renninger, K. A., & Su, S. (2012). Interest and its development. In R. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 167– 187). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 351

Rotgans, J. I., & Schmidt, H. G. (2011). Situational interest and academic achievement in the active-­learning classroom. Learning and Instruction, 21, 58–67. Rotgans, J. I., & Schmidt, H. G. (2014). Situational interest and learning: Thirst for knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 32, 37–50. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-­ determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.). (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance. New York: Academic Press. Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the missing motivator in self-­regulation. European Psychologist, 10, 175–186. Schiefele, U. (2001). The role of interest in motivation and learning. In J. M. Collis & S. Messick (Eds.), Intelligence and personality: Bridging the gap in theory and measurement (pp. 163–194). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 197– 222). New York: Routledge. Schmidt, H. G., Rotgans, J. I., & Yew, E. H. (2011). The process of problem-­based learning: What works and why. Medical Education, 45, 792–806. Schraw, G., & Lehmann, S. (2001). Situational interest: A review of literature and directions for further research. Educational Psychological Review, 13, 23–52. Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2010). Motivation in education: Theory, research and application. London: Pearson. Seidel, T., & Prenzel, M. (2006). Stability of teaching patterns in physics instruction: Findings from a video study. Learning and Instruction, 16, 228–240. Tanaka, A., & Murayama, K. (2014). Within-­ person analyses of situational interest and boredom: Interactions between task-­ specific perceptions and achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 1122–1134. Thoman, D. B., Smith, J. L., & Silvia, P. (2011). The resource replenishment function of interest. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 592–599. Tibbetts, Y., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., & Canning, E. A. (2016). Broadening participation in the life sciences with social–­ psychological interventions. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 15(3), es4. Tröbst, S., Kleickmann, T., Lange-­S chubert, K., Rothkopf, A., & Möller, K. (2016). Instruction and students’ declining interest in science: An

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM

352 analysis of German fourth- and sixth-grade classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 162–193. Tsai, Y. M., Kunter, M., Lütke, O., Trautwein, U., & Ryan, M. R. (2008). What makes lessons interesting?: The roles of situational and individual factors in three school subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 460–472. Tulis, M., & Fulmer, S. M. (2013). Students’ motivational and emotional experiences and their relationship to persistence during academic challenge in mathematics and reading. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 35–46. Turner, J. C. (2010). Unfinished business: Putting motivation theory to the “Classroom Test.” In S. Karabenick & T. Urdan (Eds.), The decade ahead: Applications and contexts of motivation and achievement (Vol. 16B, pp. 109– 138). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group. Walker, A., & Leary, H. (2009). A problem based learning meta-­ analysis: Differences across problem types, implementation types, disciplines, and assessment levels. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-­Based Learning, 3, 12–43. Walkington, C. A. (2013). Using adaptive learning technologies to personalize instruction to student interests: The impact of relevant contexts on performance and learning outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 932– 945.

Elliot_HbkCmptncMtvtn2E.indb 352

III.  RELEVANT PROCESSES Walkington, C., & Bernacki, M. L. (2014). Motivating students by “personalizing” learning around individual interests: A consideration of theory, design, and implementation issues. Advances in Motivation and Achievement, 18, 139–176. Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise interventions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 73–82. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–318. Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy–­value theory of achievement motivation: A developmental perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 49–78. Wijnia, L., Loyens, S. M., & Derous, E. (2011). Investigating effects of problem-­based versus lecture-­ based learning environments on student motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 101–113. Wijnia, L., Loyens, S. M., Derous, E., & Schmidt, H. G. (2014). Do students’ topic interest and tutors’ instructional style matter in problem-­ based learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 919–933. Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M., Paunesku, D., Walton, G., Spitzer, B., D’Mello, S., et al. (2014). Boring but important: A self-­ transcendent purpose for learning fosters academic self-­ regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 559–580.

11/22/2016 4:01:22 PM