Cognitive aging and Alzheimer's disease

2 downloads 63 Views 382KB Size Report
Nov 2, 2004 - cognitive deficits during life, neuritic plaques and neuro- fibrillary tangles ..... 2001;357:169–75. 54 Perry EK, Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, et al.
343

REVIEW

Cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease R Vandenberghe, J Tournoy ............................................................................................................................... Postgrad Med J 2005;81:343–352. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.2004.028290

Cognitive aging and clinically probable Alzheimer’s disease can be discriminated by means of clinical and neuropsychological testing, and structural and functional imaging techniques. Research at the level of cognitive brain systems and at the molecular level provides exciting new insights into the relation between aging and neurodegeneration. The advances at the clinical and at the basic research levels are necessary if we wish to meet the formidable challenge that the increasing prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease poses to the medical community. ...........................................................................

A

See end of article for authors’ affiliations ....................... Correspondence to: Professor R Vandenberghe, Neurology Department, University Hospital Gasthuisberg, Herestraat 49, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; rik.vandenberghe@uz. kuleuven.ac.be Submitted 31 August 2004 Accepted 2 November 2004 .......................

mong the neurodegenerative diseases, clinically probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has the highest prevalence, estimated at 1.450/100 000.1 Between 65 and 69 years of age, its prevalence is estimated at 1.53%.2 From the age of 60 onwards up to 80 years of age, incidence and prevalence of AD increases exponentially with age, doubling every five years.2 3 Between the age of 80 and 85 the prevalence is as high as 25%–30% of the population.4 The distinction between this highly prevalent disorder and normal aging will be the focus of this review. Given the demographic evolution, AD will pose a challenge over the decades to come all over the world. The distinction between normal aging and AD is one first step if we want to combat this disease efficiently. This review draws a new face of AD: a treatable disorder that evolves insidiously over many years and that can be diagnosed at an early stage by means of positive diagnostic features. This contrasts with a more traditional view of AD as an exclusion diagnosis of an untreatable disorder associated with impairment of basic activities of daily living. We will adopt an evidence based approach (table 1). On several occasions positive likelihood ratios will be used. This permits the combination of sensitivity and specificity into one value (table 1). Positive likelihood ratios between 2 and 5 imply small (but sometimes important) shifts between pre-test and post-test probabilities and values between 5 and 10 moderate shifts. Values above 10 imply conclusive changes to the pre-test probability.

DISCRIMINATING NORMAL AGING FROM PROBABLE AD The distinction between age related cognitive decline and probable AD has important clinical implications. Probable AD is a treatable disorder5–9 while no evidence exists at the moment that medical treatment is beneficial for age

related cognitive decline. Counselling about diagnosis and prognosis also differs between cognitive aging and probable AD. The transition from normal aging to probable AD, however, is not categorical. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) refers to this transition zone from normal aging to probable AD.10

Clinical features

Cognitive aging Only a minority of aged people maintain their peak cognitive performance level during senescence. They experience successful aging.11 In most healthy people above the age of 50, episodic memory (table 2) declines with respect to the person’s prior performance.12 13 Age related episodic memory decline typically affects free recall more than recognition (familiarity judgment).14 15 Other cognitive changes associated with normal aging are a decrease in cognitive processing speed16 and executive functions (table 2). Typically, cognitive aging does not affect cognition globally but leaves specific cognitive domains intact, for example, familiarity judgement. In this review, we refer to this pattern of age related cognitive changes as cognitive aging. On its own a subjective memory complaint does not increase the risk of developing probable AD.17 In a hierarchical regression model, subjective memory complaints were explained significantly more by mood than by actual memory performance on cognitive tests.17 When a patient presents with a subjective memory complaint, two key questions are: ‘‘Have your relatives, Table 1 Evidence based medicine terminology

Test positive Test negative

Disease positive

Disease negative

a c

b d

Specificity = d/(b+d)); sensitivity = a/(a+c); positive likelihood ratio = sensitivity/ (12specificity); positive predictive value = a/(a+b); negative predictive value = d/ (c+d).

Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; MMSE, mini-mental state examination; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living; PET, positron emission tomography; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; CT, computed tomography; LTP, long term potentiation; CREB, cAMP response element binding protein; PKA, protein kinase A; APP, amyloid precursor protein; KPI, kunitz protease inhibitor; AVF, animal verbal fluency

www.postgradmedj.com

344

Vandenberghe, Tournoy

friends or colleagues noticed any change?’’, and ‘‘Does the cognitive change impact on the way you carry out daily activities (professional, leisure, finances, shopping, car driving, etc)?’’A positive answer increases the likelihood that the patient suffers from an objective cognitive deficit, possibly related to degenerative disease, rather than a subjective perception of age related memory decline. A diagnosis of age related memory decline requires exclusion of alternative explanations, such as vascular brain lesions, obstructive sleep apnea syndrome, depression, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, diabetes mellitus, or cardiac failure.18 The drug list must be systematically reviewed for drugs that are used commonly in the elderly population and that exert minor or major cognitive side effects, such as benzodiazepines used during the day, tricyclic antidepressants, opioid derivatives, and centrally active antihistaminergic drugs.

Mild cognitive impairment The distinction between cognitive aging and a pathological deficit may be subtle. A standardised systematic neuropsychological assessment may be necessary to distinguish between cognitive aging and a pathological deficit. A prospective longitudinal study of the diagnostic accuracy of neuropsychological tests in patients referred for memory loss determined which neuropsychological tests discriminated best between patients who developed probable AD and those who remained non-demented over a two year follow up period.19 Classic neuropsychological episodic memory tests, which require the learning and recalling of lists of words, elements of a story, or series of abstract visual designs, had a positive likelihood ratio of about 12,19 20 corresponding to a moderate to large change from pre-test to post-test probability (table 1). Could we increase the diagnostic value by doing repeat examination at, for example, one year intervals and carrying out within subject comparisons? There are two caveats. The standard error of the difference between the initial and the repeat test is the square root of two times higher than the standard error of the test itself. Secondly, repeat assessments in normal controls suffer from a comparatively high within subject variance from one test to another. Specific norms of longitudinal change are necessary for each of the tests to discover if a person’s change falls within a normal range.21 Despite these caveats, if repeat neuropsychological assessment with a one year interval shows significant deterioration in a patient classified as MCI or, on the other hand, improvement or stabilisation, this is clinically valuable information regarding the risk of conversion to probable AD.22 If a pathological memory deficit is present, if there is no significant impairment of instrumental activities of daily living, and if other cognitive domains are intact and if

Key point Cognitive aging refers to age related decline that selectively affects specific cognitive processes, including delayed recall, processing speed, and executive functions.

identifiable medical or psychiatric causes have been excluded, the patient may be diagnosed as MCI.23 A diagnosis of MCI implies a risk of conversion to probable AD of 15% per year.10 MCI patients should therefore undergo regular (for example, six monthly) clinical or neuropsychological follow up, to ensure timely diagnosis of conversion to probable AD.23 No treatment with verified efficacy exists for MCI.

Clinically probable AD A diagnosis of probable AD requires the presence of an acquired and progressive episodic memory deficit (table 2) combined with impairment in at least one other cognitive domain and associated with a significant impact on instrumental activities of daily living (NINCDS-ADRDA24 or DSM-IV criteria25). The cognitive deficits should not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium.25 If the patient suffers from concomitant diseases that may contribute to the cognitive deficits, the patient falls under the category of clinically possible AD. Episodic memory impairment is a cornerstone of the diagnosis of probable AD24 25 (table 2). Typical episodic memory complaints are: I have to re-read previous passages in books more frequently than before, I have difficulty retrieving my parked car, or I forget where I was seated or what I went to look for. Patients may frequently ask the same questions, reiterate identical stories, or not remember recent social events they attended. Clinically useful ways to test episodic memory are the three words recall item and the orientation items of the mini-mental state examination (MMSE)26 or the three words three shapes test.27 In addition to episodic memory, at least one other cognitive domain must be affected to meet criteria. Domains that are frequently affected at an early stage are executive functions,28 word

Key point Criteria for mild cognitive impairment are the presence of a subjective memory complaint in a person above 50 years of age, evidence of an objective pathological episodic memory deficit, minimal impact on instrumental daily activities, and no alternative explanations (medication, comorbidity, depression).

Table 2 Definitions of some of the important cognitive domains that are affected early in Alzheimer’s disease Episodic memory Typical clinical test Neuroanatomy Semantic memory Typical clinical test Neuroanatomy Executive functions Typical clinical test Neuroanatomy Working memory Typical clinical test Neuroanatomy

www.postgradmedj.com

Memory for personally experienced events in a particular temporal and spatial context Three words recall of MMSE Hippocampal formation, prefrontal cortex, diencephalon Knowledge of public events, words, and of the associations between concepts Animal verbal fluency Anterior temporal lobe, distributed neocortical representation Coordination of multiple cognitive processes Animal verbal fluency Prefrontal cortex Ability to manipulate short term memory representations Backward digit span Prefrontal cortex

Cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease

finding, and semantic memory.29 The MMSE also contains items for working memory (serial 7 and spelling backwards), confrontation naming (pen and watch), and constructional abilities (overlapping pentagons). According to a prospective study comparing patients with dementia and normal controls, an MMSE cut off score of 23 of 30 had a positive likelihood ratio of 8.4.30 A retrospective study compared probable AD patients visiting a memory clinic with normal controls and found that MMSE with a cut off of 25–26 of 30 distinguished the two groups with a positive likelihood ratio of 37.31 An MMSE score below 24 generally points to the presence of cognitive impairment. A score above 24 however does not exclude that the patient suffers from a clinically significant, objective cognitive deficit. In these cases the diagnosis of memory and other cognitive deficits relies on clinical tests such as the animal verbal fluency (AVF) or MMSE subitems (for example, the three words recall) as well as on neuropsychological assessment. The AVF test is a classic clinical test that probes executive functions as well as semantic memory32 (table 2). Subjects have to enumerate as many animals as possible within one minute. This test does not rely on episodic memory and is impaired comparatively early in the course of AD.33 Clinically, a score below 12 on the AVF is nearly always abnormal. A retrospective study in normal controls and probable AD patients yielded a positive likelihood ratio for the AVF of 14.3.34 Other examples of clinically useful tests are the clock drawing test23 and the 15 item version of the Boston naming test.32 A third criterion for the diagnosis of probable AD is significant impairment of instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), including the ability to handle finances, drive safely, or go shopping. A retrospective necropsy study conducted by a consortium of US Alzheimer’s disease centres of 1833 cases with premortem diagnosis of probable or possible AD and 355 non-AD patients showed that clinical consensus criteria had a positive predictive value (table 1) of 90% but a negative predictive value of only 64%. This means that a considerable number of patients with neuropathologically definite AD are not diagnosed as Alzheimer patients, even at academic research centres.35 Comparable estimates for a combined diagnosis of probable and possible AD by NINCDS-ADRDA criteria were obtained in the prospective community based Oxford project to investigate memory and aging study.36 37 In that study of 19 probable AD patients, 39 possible AD patients, 7 non-AD demented patients, and 9 controls, the positive predictive value of the NINCDS criteria of probable AD with respect to a pathological diagnosis of AD (CERAD criteria38) was 100% and the negative predictive value 50%. The positive predictive value of the criteria for probable AD or possible AD grouped together was 88% and the negative predictive value 69%.36 At the time of initial evaluation of a patient with cognitive complaints, a blood examination is recommended.39 This should include complete blood count, serum electrolytes, blood urea/creatinine, sedimentation, calcium and phosphorus, liver function tests, vitamin B12, and thyroid stimulating hormone (TSH). Vitamin B12 deficiency or lowering of TSH are common in the elderly population. These abnormalities however rarely cause cognitive deficits as their sole and primary clinical manifestation.39

Brain pathology

Cognitive aging In rat hippocampus and prefrontal cortex stereological postmortem studies have not provided clear proof of age related neuronal loss.40 In humans, the number and length of dendritic segments in specific layers of dorsolateral prefrontal

345

Figure 1 Neuroanatomy of episodic memory. Green: medial temporal cortex; red: prefrontal cortex; yellow: Basal forebrain (including nucleus basalis of Meynert among other structures); brown: dorsomedial thalamus. (A) Transparant view through the brain; (B) transverse MRI sections, from top to bottom.

cortex decreases with age, together with a decrease in dendritic spine and synaptic density.41 In the non-demented elderly population, the entorhinal cortex and the basal forebrain (fig 1) display diffuse plaques as well as neurofibrillary tangles or pre-tangle tau pathology. It is possible that these lesions also contribute to cognitive aging.42

Clinically probable AD Neurodegenerative diseases are characterised by the progressive loss of neurons with a high neuroanatomical selectivity at the initial stages. This neuroanatomical selectivity determines the clinical manifestations. The earliest changes in AD are in the entorhinal cortex43 44 (fig 1), a structure that lies just laterally to the hippocampus and provides input to the

Key point Criteria for a diagnosis of clinically probable Alzheimer’s disease are the presence of an episodic memory deficit in combination with impairment of at least one other cognitive domain. Frequently affected domains at an early stage are executive functions and word finding. There must be a significant impact on activities of daily living and no alternative explanations (delirium, medication, comorbidity, depression).

www.postgradmedj.com

346

hippocampus.45 This explains why episodic memory deficits play such a prominent part in the definition of AD. From the entorhinal cortex, disease spreads to the hippocampus and subsequently to the inferotemporal and lateral temporal cortex and other neocortical association areas. This explains the impairment of other cognitive domains apart from episodic memory. This hierarchical spread of AD has been divided into six stages by Braak and Braak.43 According to this proposal, the topography and intensity of the Alzheimer lesions parallels different stages in the clinical evolution.43 Several criteria have been put forward for a pathological diagnosis of definite AD,38 43 46 with varying emphasis on the presence of neuritic plaques or neurofibrillary tangles (fig 2). Neuritic plaques (fig 2A) consist of the extracellular deposition of congophilic, amyloid material that can be immunostained with antibodies against Ab42 and also with antibodies against paired helical filament t (PHF t).47 Neuritic plaques must be distinguished from diffuse plaques, which only stain for Ab42 and are present in the aging brain from the age of 40 onwards. Neurofibrillary tangles (fig 2B) represent intracellular paired helical filaments of hyperphosphorylated protein. They can be visualised with silver and thioflavin S staining and with immunostaining for PHFt. Neurofibrillary tangles are nearly always present in the entorhinal cortex above the age of 75.44 48 Brains from patients with probable AD also show vascular lesions, such as cerebral amyloid angiopathy and cerebral

Vandenberghe, Tournoy

endothelial degeneration.49 The additional presence of lacunar infarcts in the basal ganglia, the thalamus, or the white matter in neuropathologically definite AD brains significantly increases the probability that the disease expressed itself clinically, with an odds ratio of 21.50 The neuropathological distinction between brains of patients that suffered from probable AD during their lifetime and brains of cognitively normal people is less clear cut than originally proposed.51 In people who do not show pathological cognitive deficits during life, neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles may be present in sufficient quantity to warrant a neuropathological diagnosis of definite AD.52 In the Medical Research Council cognitive function and aging (MRC-CFAS) study, a prospective community based necropsy study, diffuse and neuritic amyloid plaques as well as neurofibrillary tangles were present in a significant percentage of cognitively intact elderly people in numbers and with a distribution sufficient to warrant a pathological diagnosis of definite AD.53 Multiple regression analysis with dementia status as outcome measure and the degree and distribution of tangles and plaques as explanatory variables misclassified one in four non-demented controls as demented.53 Neurochemically, AD is associated with neuronal loss in the nucleus basalis of Meynert leading to cholinergic depletion.54 According to the cholinergic theory, this depletion underlies the episodic memory and attentional deficits.55 This influential theory has driven the development of cholinesterase inhibitors as a treatment for probable AD. In mild to moderate disease stages (MMSE 10 to 24 of 30), cholinesterase inhibitors (donepezil, galantamine, and rivastigmine) favourably affect cognition, activities of daily living, and behavioural symptoms.5 7 9 56 The difference between patients receiving active treatment or receiving placebo is sufficiently large to be detectable by means of semistructured interviews of patients and caregivers by clinicians blinded for the treatment (such as the clinician interview based impression of change).5 7 9 56

Mild cognitive impairment Necropsy studies in MCI so far have mostly shown pathological features consistent with the diagnosis of early stage AD, corresponding to Braak and Braak stages 3 to 4 (limbic stages).42 57 Neurochemically, no cholinergic depletion was found in neocortex58 or in the hippocampal formation59 in patients who died with a diagnosis of MCI or early probable AD. This has cast doubt on the validity of the cholinergic theory55 at an early disease stage. In a recent necropsy study of the nucleus basalis, the important source of cholinergic innervation for the cortex, Mesulam et al compared a group of five MCI patients and seven age matched controls: the cholinergic neurons of the nucleus basalis showed significantly more tangles and pre-tangles than the age matched controls.42 This suggests that cholinergic depletion does have a role at the pre-dementia stage in structures that are most sensitive to the disease process.

Key point

Figure 2 Classic pathological hallmarks for Alzheimer’s disease. (A) Neuritic plaques, stained with anti-Ab42 antibodies; (B) neuritic plaque and neurofibrillary tangles, stained with paired helical filament t antibody.

www.postgradmedj.com

Several sets of consensus criteria exist for a diagnosis of pathologically definite AD. Neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles are the hallmarks of AD but also occur in normal aging. Prospective double blind studies that correlate the clinical diagnosis with pathological diagnosis show that brain necropsy findings do not always accurately predict who was demented or cognitively normal during lifetime.

Cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease

Key point Pathological findings support the notion that MCI is a transition zone between normal aging and AD.

In vivo structural and functional imaging

Cognitive aging Recent technical advances permit the investigation of age related brain changes in humans during lifetime. In a large cross sectional structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study of 465 normal adult human brains, medial temporal structures such as the hippocampus or the entorhinal cortex did not show measurable diminution of volume.60 This corresponds to recent stereological findings in rat brains that did not show age related neuronal loss in the hippocampus.61 A second in vivo brain imaging technique, positron emission tomography (PET), permits the measurement of regional brain glucose metabolism (18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET) as well as the integrity of neurotransmitter systems. Aging is associated with a decline in glucose metabolism in specific brain regions, such as the anterior cingulate and perisylvian cortex62 and with changes in neurotransmitter systems, such as a decrease in D2 dopamine receptors and presynaptic dopamine transporters.63 Given the projections of the dopaminergic system to prefrontal cortex (fig 1), this dopaminergic decrease may be related to the decrease in mental flexibility that occurs with aging (table 2). A different methodology, functional MRI (fMRI), permits the measurement of brain activation related to motor, sensory, or cognitive processes. Most studies of age related changes in cognitive brain systems have relied on cross sectional comparisons between comparatively small numbers of younger and elderly subjects. Potential confounders in functional imaging studies of aging are between subject differences in task performance and the adoption of task solving strategies that differ from those used by the young subjects. Some of the general principles of age related reorganisation are: (1) Loss of inter-hemispheric gradients.64 65 For instance, in younger subjects the left prefrontal cortex is more active during encoding and the right prefrontal cortex during retrieval. In older subjects this hemispheric encoding/ retrieval asymmetry is much less pronounced.65 (2) Context dependency of age related changes. A given region may be less active when two specific conditions are compared but may be normally activated in a contrast between two other conditions.66 The dynamic nature of the age related changes differs fundamentally from the context independent changes studied with structural MRI. (3) Loss of cognitive specificity of activity patterns.66 67 For instance, while the processing of location and facial identity follows a dorsal and a ventral processing stream, respectively, in the younger group, this distinction is less clear in older subjects.67 This loss of specificity could be considered as a reversal of the functional differentiation during the developmental stage.

Key point Cognitive aging is not merely associated with decreases in brain activations but also with activity increases and functional reorganisation

347

Clinically probable AD Only in 1%–5% of patients who are clinically suspected to suffer from probable AD and have no clinical indication of an alternative cause does computed tomography (CT) disclose an alternative cause, such as a tumour, silent large vessel stroke, or subdural haematoma.68 As the detection of neoplasms or subdural haematoma significantly changes diagnosis and treatment, one brain scan in the routine initial evaluation of patients with dementia is appropriate.39 MRI allows for accurate volumetric measurements of medial temporal structures, such as the hippocampus. As medial temporal neuronal loss is an important feature of AD, MRI can be used to corroborate the positive diagnosis of

Figure 3 (A, B) Role of structural MRI as an exclusionary tool. Both cases presented with a clinical picture typical for probable AD. (A) A 83 year old man presents with memory complaints without significant impact on IADL. These cognitive changes had been apparent for two months. MMSE score is 23/30 and animal verbal fluency 6. The clock drawing test is normal. On clinical neurological examination there are discrete extrapiramidal signs to the right. MRI shows a comparatively recent chronic subdural haematoma. (B) A 72 years old woman presents with memory complaints as well as behavioural changes (irritability and mood swings) without significant impact on IADL. MMSE score is 24/30 (delayed recall sub-item 0/3); animal verbal fluency 10, clock drawing test is normal. MRI shows a sphenoidal meningeoma with pressure on the basal forebrain. (C, D, E, F) Various grades of hippocampal atrophy.70 (C) Normal hippocampal volume. (D) Grade 1: the fissura choroidea is dilated. (E) A 75 year old woman has had memory complaints for one year, with significant impact on IADL. MMSE score is 19, animal verbal fluency 7, months backwards is impossible. During the clock drawing test the patient counts from 12 onwards and writes these numbers in the circle. The overlapping pentagon test of the MMSE is impossible. The 15 item version of the Boston naming test: 7/15. Clinically the patient fulfils the criteria of probable AD. Coronal MRI sections show hippocampal atrophy grade 2: The fissura choroidea and the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle are dilated. (F) Grade 4: severe widening of the fissura choroidea, of the temporal horn, and flattening of the hippocampus.

www.postgradmedj.com

348

Vandenberghe, Tournoy

Figure 4 A 58 year old woman was admitted to the emergency department after syncope. Since the age of 57 she suffered from progressive apraxia and aphasia and increased apathy. She could no longer handle money, organise shopping, or cooking. Fluorodeoxyglucose PET scan shows severe hypometabolism of parietal, frontal, and temporal association cortex with relative sparing of the primary visual and motor cortex. The left hemisphere is predominantly affected than the right.

probable AD (for review see Scheltens et al68), and not only as a tool to exclude alternative explanations (fig 3 A and B). In early stage AD (MMSE.21) the percentage of patients with a normalised total hippocampal volume below the 50th centile compared with normal controls was 97%. Fifty eight per cent was situated below the 5th centile.69 Visual ratings of medial temporal lobe atrophy in probable AD and healthy controls reach a sensitivity of 81% and a specificity of 67%70 (fig 3 C, D, E, F). Hippocampal volume correlates with disease stage, as measured with MMSE or neuropathology.71 Hippocampal atrophy does not differentiate AD from other types of dementia.71 In one longitudinal three year study, the mean annualised rate of hippocampal atrophy was 1.73% in stable controls and 3.5% in patients with probable AD.72 A potential application lies in the use of hippocampal atrophy as a surrogate marker in therapeutic trials.73 A second MRI investigation in the diagnosis of AD is the evaluation of the vascular burden. The presence of lacunar infarcts on MRI by no means excludes a diagnosis of probable AD.50 When the vascular lesions are extensive, for example, encompassing more than one fourth of the white matter, or strategically localised, for example, in the dorsomedial thalamus, the patient may be classified as clinically probable vascular dementia according to the NINDS-AIREN criteria74 rather than probable AD under the condition that the patient also has clinical signs of neurological lateralisation and a stepwise progressive disease course.74 A prospective community based necropsy study in 80 patients with an MMSE below 24 showed that the specificity of the NINDS-AIREN criteria was 95% with a sensitivity of only 43%.75 While structural MRI studies have focused on hippocampal changes, FDG PET principally provides an image of the involvement of neocortical association areas. The characteristic FDG PET pattern in probable AD is a decline of FDG uptake in posterior cingulate, temporoparietal, and lateral frontal association cortex (fig 4). This decline correlates with dementia severity.62 In patients with mild probable AD (MMSE above 23) compared with non-demented controls the sensitivity was 84% at a specificity of 93%.62 Among 128 patients with cognitive decline who underwent an FDG PET during lifetime as well as postmortem neuropathological diagnosis, sensitivity of FDG PET for the diagnosis of AD compared with other forms of dementia was 94% and specificity 73%.76 Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) may also show characteristic patterns of perfusion changes that distinguish AD from normal controls.

www.postgradmedj.com

A neuropathologically verified series of 80 AD patients, 24 non-AD dementia patients, and 14 controls showed a sensitivity of 89% with a specificity of only 60%. The positive predictive value of SPECT for the discrimination between AD and normal controls was 81% and the negative predictive value 74%.77 Functional MRI studies comparing probable AD with age matched controls showed higher activity in prefrontal cortex during episodic and semantic memory in early stage AD.78 The activity increases correlated positively with performance, suggesting that they represent compensatory mechanisms.78 Activity levels in the hippocampus during episodic memory tasks are decreased in AD.13

Mild cognitive impairment A structural quantitative hippocampal volumetry study in 80 MCI patients showed that according to a lifetable analysis, probable AD free survival differed significantly between MCI patients with volumes above the 50th centile (n = 13), between the 50th and the 1st percentile (n = 54), and those lower than the 1st percentile (n = 13). In the first group 75% of patients did not convert to probable AD after a five year period while in the second group this number was 25%.79 80 Cellular biology

Cognitive aging In mouse models of aging the age related decline in spatial memory is associated with a reduction of long term potentiation (LTP), a neurophysiological mechanism for episodic memory.61 Specifically, the late phase of LTP is affected. This late phase is mediated by cAMP and proteine kinase A (PKA) and is associated with RNA and protein synthesis (for review see Nguyen and Woo81) (fig 5). This pathway can be activated by an increased Ca influx (fig 5A) or by activation of guanine nucleotide binding regulatory protein (G protein) (fig 5B). This induces cytosolic cAMP,

Key point Hippocampal volumes differ significantly between elderly people and patients who have probable AD or who are going to develop probable AD. There is however a pronounced degree of overlap in volumes so that predictions at the individual level are difficult.

Cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease

349

Figure 5 The cAMP/PKA signalling cascade is principally activated by two mechanisms. The first (A) involves calcium and calmodulin (Ca/CaM). Influx of calcium through for instance NMDA receptors stimulates Ca/CaM sensitive adenylyl cyclase, which synthesises cAMP. The second (B) mechanism for activation of cAMP/PKA signalling entails binding of chemical transmitters and hormones to their receptors, followed by stimulation of adenylyl cyclase by guanine nucleotide binding regulatory proteins (G proteins). Cytosolic cAMP can bind to regulatory subunits (R) of protein kinase A (PKA) holoenzymes. This releases free catalytic subunits (C) that translocate to the nucleus and phosphorylate downstream proteins such as cAMP response element binding protein (CREB). This phosphorylation can start transcription of cAMP response elements (CRE) associated genes.

which leads in its turn to activation of calcium/calmodulin dependent kinase IV or protein kinase A (PKA), which then translocates to the nucleus and phosphorylates cAMP response element binding protein (CREB). phosphoCREB in turn activates gene transcription necessary for memory and neuronal plasticity. Expression of CREB and coactivators of CREB as well as CREB DNA binding activity decrease significantly with aging.82 These age related molecular changes in cognitive brain systems differ between brain regions. Advancing age results in the reduction of cAMP/PKA signalling in hippocampus where the opposite can be shown for prefrontal cortex. This neuroanatomical specificity has its consequences for the development of therapeutic agents for age related memory decline. Activation of cAMP/PKA through phosphodiesterase inhibitors or dopamine 5 receptor agonists may be beneficial for hippocampus dependent age related memory impairment61 while agents that increase PKA activity impair prefrontal cortical function in aged rats and monkeys with prefrontal cortical deficits.83

Clinically probable AD Trisomy 21, mutations of the amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene and of presenilin 1 and 2 lead to AD. In these familial disorders Ab 42 is increased because the total production of Ab, the ratio of Ab 42 over Ab 40, or its fibrillogenetic propensity are increased. The Ab 40–42 is proteolytically released from a larger amyloid precursor protein (APP) by two sequential cleavages (fig 6).

N N

A first cleavage occurs in the extracellular domain and is exerted by b secretase, identied as BACE1, an aspartic protease.84 The second cleavage is an intramembraneous cleavage by the c -secretase complex. This cleavage requires at least three other members (aph 1, pen 2, and nicastrin) in addition to the proteolytic active presenilins.

According to the amyloid cascade hypothesis, the neurotoxic Ab 42 is the culprit of both sporadic and familial AD. Ab is a highly hydrophobic peptide that can aggregate into oligomers, protobrils, and eventually can precipitate in the characterising amyloid plaques. The amyloid cascade hypothesis provides several potential targets for drug development, such as b-secretase inhibitors85 or active immunisation against amyloid plaques.86 Excitotoxicity refers to the damage evoked by overstimulation of the glutamatergic N-methyl D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors. This over-excitation provides a rationale for treating probable AD patients by means of memantine, a reversible competitive NMDA receptor antagonist. Memantine has verified efficacy in patients with probable AD and an MMSE between 3 and 14.8 The effect size, measured by means of cognitive scales, is somewhat smaller than that reported with cholinesterase inhibitors in mild to moderate AD patients.8

AGING AS A RISK FACTOR FOR NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASE The strongest risk factor for probable AD is age. Over our lifespan the consequences of noxious events accumulate and may progressively affect the brain and increase the risk of

Key point Morphometry of the hippocampus does not show clear agerelated neuronal loss in the hippocampus. At the molecular level, however, pathways that underlie memory consolidation undergo age related changes that may account for cognitive aging patterns.

Key point The molecular pathway that gives rise to amyloid deposition is known in reasonable detail and offers multiple potential targets for drug treatment.

www.postgradmedj.com

350

Vandenberghe, Tournoy

Key point

Key references

Mid-life hypertension is a significant risk factor for probable AD

N N

neurodegenerative disease (indirect consequence of aging). Alternatively, the aging process by itself may be associated with changes that increase the risk of AD (direct effect of aging). Indirect consequences of aging Epidemiological studies provide evidence for an association between probable AD and mid-life hypertension,87–89 diabetes mellitus,89 90 and hyperhomocysteinaemia.91 Clinical stroke is also an important risk factor for the development of AD. In the absence of clinical stroke, the presence of white matter hyperintensities on T2 MRI also increases the risk of dementia.92 Direct consequences of aging

Aging and the amyloid cascade hypothesis In humans, monkeys, and transgenic mice, Ab levels increase with age.93 Logically, this could happen through three major mechanisms: (1) Increased production of Ab and hence, of Ab 42 (2) Increased ratios of Ab 42 over Ab 40 (3) Reduced clearance of Ab 42 Increased production of total Ab or Ab 42.

Total APP mRNA or protein brain levels do not increase with age94 but there is a significant relative increase in mRNA for two APP isoforms as a result of alternative splicing (kunitz protease inhibitor (KPI) harbouring APP751 and 770).95 These KPI containing APP isoforms comparatively increase the production of Ab.96 Activity of b-secretase, a necessary enzymatic step in the production of Ab (fig 6), also increases with age in the cortices of both humans and monkeys because of age related post-translational changes.93

N N N

Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al. Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Neurology 2001;56:1143–53. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–59. Neuropathology group of the Medical Research Council cognitive function and ageing study. Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet 2001;357:169–75. Francis PT, Palmer AM, Snape M, et al. The cholinergic hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: a review of progress. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66: 137–47. Scheltens P, Fox N, Barkhof F, et al. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: beyond exclusion. Lancet Neurol 2002;1:13–21.

Reduced Ab clearance

Neprilysin and insulin degrading enzyme (IDE) are two important Ab degrading enzymes.97 98 The age related changes in these enzymes differ between regions and no clear picture has emerged until now. A genetic polymorphism of apolipoprotein E(e4–e4 and e4–e3) is associated with an earlier onset of the AD in a dose dependent manner.35 Apolipoprotein E binds to the VLDL related protein receptor and also to amyloid plaques. One possible explanation for the association between AD and the apolipoprotein E polymorphism is that the e 4 polymorphism is associated with a reduced clearance of Ab 42. Not only does Ab 42 increase with age,99 the toxicity of Ab may also augment with age.100 The gradual load of toxic Ab species can further induce tauopathy, inflammatory changes, and oxidative stress, events that are already setting off Figure 6 The amyloid cascade hypothesis states that the generation of b amyloid (A) derived from the amyloid precursor protein (APP) triggers multiple secondary events that eventually lead to neuronal cell death. Which forms of Ab contribute to the toxicity is a subject of debate (question marks in figure).

www.postgradmedj.com

Cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease

in normal aged brains and could intensify Ab induced pathology.

CONCLUSION The discrimination between cognitive aging and probable AD has important implications for treatment and also fundamentally changes the information provided to the patient and the caregiver concerning diagnosis and prognosis. In our experience, coping by the patient or the caregiver may significantly improve with early clarification of the nature of the disease that underlies cognitive and behavioural changes. At a moderate to severe disease stage clinical tests of cognitive functioning may suffice together with brain imaging and blood examination to establish the diagnosis. In early probable AD or in MCI, neuropsychological assessment, hippocampal MRI volumetry, and FDG PET have confirmed positive diagnostic value. They may provide a means to diagnose neurodegenerative diseases before massive and irreversible neuronal loss has taken place. These tools may have an important role in the development of treatment aimed at prolonging neuronal survival. Given the association between AD and aging, delaying the onset of probable AD would significantly reduce its overall prevalence.

MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS (TRUE(T)/FALSE (F); ANSWERS AT END OF REFERENCES 1. 2. 3.

4. 5.

6. 7. 8.

Neuropathological examination alone is the gold standard for the diagnosis of AD. Normal aging is not associated with hippocampal atrophy, in contrast with probable AD. Initial assessment of a patient with a pathological memory deficit requires, among other tests, structural brain imaging, and determination of vitamin B12 and TSH. A modern diagnosis of probable AD relies on technical investigations. The animal verbal fluency test has confirmed diagnostic accuracy for the discrimination between normal aging and probable AD. Hippocampal atrophy allows for the distinction between different dementia subtypes. Aging inevitably leads to AD. Mid-life hypertension is a risk factor for cognitive decline in later life stages.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS RV is a clinical investigator and JT a research fellow of the Fund for Scientic Research, Flanders. The authors acknowledge the support of the Queen Elisabeth Medical Foundation. .....................

Authors’ affiliations

R Vandenberghe, Neurology Department, University Hospital Gasthuisberg, Leuven, Belgium J Tournoy, Geriatrics Department, University Hospital Gasthuisberg R Vandenberghe, Laboratorium Hersenen en Cognitie, Afdeling Experimentele Neurologie, KU Leuven J Tournoy, Centre for Human Genetics, Neuronal Cell Biology Laboratory, KU Leuven and Flanders Interuniversity Institute for Biotechnology, KU Leuven Conflicts of interest: none declared.

REFERENCES 1 Prusiner S. Shattuck lecture: Neurodegenerative diseases and prions. N Engl J Med 2001;344:1516–26. 2 Preston G. Dementia in elderly adults. Prevalence and institutionalization. J Gerontol 1986;41:261–7.

351

3 Brayne C, Gill C, Hupert FA, et al. Incidence of clinically diagnosed subtypes of dementia in an elderly population. Br J Psychiatry 1995;167:255–62. 4 Rocca WA, Hofman A, Brayne C, et al. Frequency and distribution of Alzheimer’s disease in Europe: a collaborative study of 1980–1990 prevalence findings. Ann Neurol 1991;30:381–90. 5 Rogers S, Farlow LMR, Doody R, et al. A 24-week, double-blind, placebocontrolled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Donepezil Study Group. Neurology 1998;50:136–45. 6 Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Wessel T, et al. Galantamine in AD: A 6-month placebo-controlled trial with a 6-months extension: the galantamine USA-1 study group. Neurology 2000;54:2261–8. 7 Corey-Bloom J, Anand R, Veach MS, for the ENA 713 B352 study group. A randomised trial evaluating the efficacy and safety of ENA-713, a new acetylcholinesterase inhibitor in patients with mild to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease. Intern J Geriatr Psychopharm 1998;1:55–65. 8 Reisberg B, Doody R, Stoffler A, et al. Memantine in moderate to severe Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 2003;348:1333–41. 9 Doody RS, Stevens JC, Beck C, et al. Practice parameter: management of dementia (an evidence-based review). Neurology 2001;56:1154–66. 10 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56:303–8. 11 Rowe J, Kahn R. Human aging: usual and successful. Science 1987;237:143–9. 12 Verhaeghen P, Vandenbroucke A, Dierckx V. Growing slower and less accurate: adult age differences in time-accuracy functions for recall and recognition from episodic memory. Exp Aging Res 1998;24:3–19. 13 Small SA, Stern Y, Tang M, et al. Selective decline in memory function among healthy elderly. Neurology 1999;52:1392–6. 14 Craik FIM, McDowd JM. Age differences in recall and recognition. J Exp Psychol Learning Mem Cogn 1987;13:474–9. 15 Grady CL, Craik FIM. Changes in memory processing with age. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2000;10:224–31. 16 Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 1996;103:403–28. 17 Smith G, Petersen R, Ivnik R, et al. Subjective memory complaints, psychological distress and longitudinal change in objective memory performance. Psychol Aging 1996;11:272–9. 18 Crook T, Bartus R, Ferris S, et al. Age-associated memory impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change: report of a National Institute of Mental Health work group. Devel Neuropsychol 1986;2:261–76. 19 Tierney CM, Szalai JP, Snow WG, et al. Prediction of probable Alzheimer’s disease in memory-impaired patients: a prospective longitudinal study. Neurology 1996;46:661–5. 20 Welsh K, Butters N, Hughes J, et al. Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease: using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch Neurol 1991;48:278–81. 21 Smith GE, Ivnik RJ. Normative neuropsychology. In: Petersen RC, ed. Mild cognitive impairment. New York: Oxford University Press, 2003:63–88. 22 Salmon DP, Thomas RG, Pay MM, et al. Alzheimer’s disease can be accurately diagnosed in very mildly impaired individuals. Neurology 2002;59:1022–8. 23 Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, et al. Practice parameter: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Neurology 2001;56:1133–42. 24 McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–94. 25 American Psychiatric Association. In: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994. 26 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98. 27 Weintraub S, Peavy GM, OConnoer M, et al. Three words-three shapes: a clinical test of memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000;22:267–78. 28 Baddeley AD, Baddeley HA, Bucks KS, et al. Attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2001;124:1492–508. 29 Locascio JL, Growdon JH, Corkin S. Cognitive test performance in detecting, staging and tracking Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 1995;52:1087–99. 30 Ritchie K, Fuhrer R. A comparative study of screening tests for senile dementia using receiver operating characteristics analysis. J Clin Epidemiol 1992;45:627–37. 31 Monsch A, Foldi N, Ermimi-Funfschilling D, et al. Improving the diagnostic accuracy of the mini mental state examination. Acta Neurol Scand 1995;92:145–50. 32 Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease. Part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159–65. 33 Canning SJD, Leach L, Stuss D, et al. Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Neurology 2004;62:556–62. 34 Monsch A, Bondi M, Salmon D, et al. Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 1992;49:1253–8. 35 Mayeux R, Saunders AM, Shea S, et al. Utility of the apolipoprotein E genotype for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 1998;338:506–11. 36 Nagy Z, Esiri MM, Hindley NJ, et al. Accuracy of clinical operational diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease in relation to different pathological diagnostic protocols. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disor 1998;9:219–26.

www.postgradmedj.com

352

37 Chui H, Lee AY. Clinical criteria for dementia subtypes. In: Qizilbash N, Schneider LS, Chui H, et al, eds. Evidence-based dementia practice. Oxford: Blackwell, 2003:106–19. 38 Mirra SS, Heyman A, McKeel D, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry of Alzheimer’s Disease. II. Standardization of the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1991;41:479–86. 39 Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al. Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Neurology 2001;56:1143–53. 40 Rapp PR, Gallagher M. Preserved neuron number in the hippocampus of aged rats with spatial learning deficits. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1996;93:9926–30. 41 Uylings HBM, de Brabander JM. Neuronal changes in normal human aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Cogn 2002;49:268–76. 42 Mesulam MM, Shaw P, Mash D, et al. Cholinergic nucleus basalis tauopathy emerges early in the aging-MCI-AD continuum. Ann Neurol 2004;55:815–28. 43 Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–59. 44 Delacourte A, David JP, Sergeant N, et al. The biochemical pathway of neurofibrillary degeneration in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1999;52:1158–65. 45 Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 1991;253:1380–6. 46 The National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute working group on diagnostic criteria for the neuropathological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Consensus recommendations for the postmortem diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 1997;18:S1–2. 47 Dickson DW. Alzheimer-Parkinson disease overlap: neuropathology. In: Clark CM, Trojanowski JQ, eds. Neurodegenerative dementias. New York, NJ: McGraw-Hill, 2000:247–59. 48 Guillozet AL, Weintraub S, Mash DC, et al. Neurofibrillary tangles, amyloid, and memory in aging and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2003;60:729–36. 49 Kalaria RA. Small vessel disease and Alzheimer’s dementia: pathological considerations. Cerebrovasc Dis 2002;13:48–52. 50 Snowdon DA, Greiner LH, Mortimer JA, et al. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s disease: the nun study. JAMA 1997;277:813–17. 51 Ball MJ, Murdoch GH. Neuropathological criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Are we really ready yet? Neurobiol Aging 1997;18:S3–12. 52 Davis DG, Schmitt FA, Wekstein DR, et al. Alzheimer neuropathologic alterations in aged cognitively intact subjects. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1999;58:376–88. 53 Neuropathology group of the Medical Research Council cognitive function and ageing study. Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet 2001;357:169–75. 54 Perry EK, Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, et al. Correlation of cholinergic abnormalities with senile plaques and mental test scores in senile dementia. BMJ 1978;2:1457–9. 55 Francis PT, Palmer AM, Snape M, et al. The cholinergic hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: a review of progress. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66:137–47. 56 Tariot PN, Solomon PR, Morris JC, et al. A 5-month, randomized, placebocontrolled trial of galantamine in AD. Neurology 2000;54:2269–76. 57 Price JL, Morris JC. Tangles and plaques in nondemented ageing and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1999;45:358–68. 58 Davis KL, Mohs RC, Marin D, et al. Cholinergic markers in elderly patients with early signs of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 1999;281:1401–6. 59 DeKosky ST, Ikonomovic MD, Styren SD, et al. Upregulation of choline acetyltransferase activity in hippocampus and frontal cortex of elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 2002;51:145–55. 60 Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, et al. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 2001;14:21–36. 61 Bach ME, Barad M, Son H, et al. Age-related defects in spatial memory are correlated with defects in the late phase of hippocampal longterm potentiation in vitro and are attenuated by drugs that enhance the cAMP signaling pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1999;96:5280–5. 62 Herholz K, Salmon E, Perani D, et al. Discrimination between Alzheimer dementia and controls by automated analysis of multicenter FDG PET. Neuroimage 2002;17:302–16. 63 Volkow ND, Ding YS, Fowler JS, et al. Dopamine transporters decrease with age. J Nucl Med 1996;37:554–9. 64 Maguire EA, Frith CD. Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain 2003;126:1511–23. 65 Cabeza R, Grady CL, Nyberg L, et al. Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci 1997;17:391–400. 66 Logan JM, Sanders AL, Snyder AZ, et al. Under-recruitment and nonselective recruitment: Dissociable neural mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron 2002;33:827–40. 67 Grady CL, Maisog JM, Horwitz B, et al. Age-related changes in cortical blood ow activation during visual processing of faces and locations. J Neurosci 1994;14:1450–62. 68 Scheltens P, Fox N, Barkhof F, et al. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: beyond exclusion. Lancet Neurol 2002;1:13–21. 69 Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, et al. Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1997;49:786–94.

www.postgradmedj.com

Vandenberghe, Tournoy

70 Scheltens P, Leys D, Barkhof F, et al. Atrophy of medial temporal lobe on MRI in probableAlzheimer’s disease and normal ageing: diagnostic value and neuropsychological correlates. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55:967–72. 71 Jack CR, Dickson DW, Parisi JE, et al. Antemortem MRIfindings correlate with hippocampal neuropathology in typical aging and dementia. Neurology 2002;58:750–7. 72 Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu Y, et al. Rates of hippocampal atrophy in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2000;55:484–9. 73 Fox N, Freeborough P. Visualisation and quantication of rates of atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 1996;348:94–7. 74 Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 1993;43:250–60. 75 Holmes C, Cairns N, Lantos P, et al. Validity of current clinical criteria for Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br J Psychiatry 1999;174:45–50. 76 Silverman DHS, Small GW, Chang CY, et al. Positron emission tomography in evaluation of dementia. JAMA 2001;286:2120–7. 77 Jobst KA, Barnetson LP, Shepstone BJ. Accurate prediction of histologically conrmed Alzheimer’s disease and the differential diagnosis of dementia: the use of NINCDS-ADRDA and DSM-III-R criteria, SPECT, X-ray CT and Apo E4 in medial temporal lobe dementias. Oxford project to investigate memory and aging. Int Psychogeriatr 1998;10:271–302. 78 Grady CL, McIntosh AR, Beig S, et al. Evidence from functional neuroimaging of a compensatory prefrontal network in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 2003;23:986–93. 79 Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, et al. Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 1999;52:1397–403. 80 Korf ESC, Wahlund LO, Visser PJ, et al. Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI predicts dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2004;63:94–100. 81 Nguyen P, Woo N. Regulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity by cyclic AMP dependent protein kinase. Prog Neurobiol 2003;71:401–37. 82 Chung YH, Kim EJ, Shin CM, et al. Age-related changes in CREB binding protein immunoreactivity in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus of rats. Brain Res 2002;956:312–18. 83 Ramos BP, Birnbaum SG, Lindenmayer I, et al. Dysregulation of protein kinase a signaling in the aged prefrontal cortex: new strategy for treating age-related cognitive decline. Neuron 2003;40:835–45. 84 Vassar R. The beta-secretase, BACE: a prime drug target for Alzheimer’s disease. J Mol Neurosci 2001;17:157–70. 85 Selkoe D, Schenk D. Molecular understanding predicts amyloid-based therapeutics. Ann Rev Pharm Toxicol 2003;43:545–84. 86 Hock C. Antibodies against beta-amyloid slow cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuron 2003;38:547–54. 87 Skoog I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, et al. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and dementia. Lancet 1996;347:1141–5. 88 Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, et al. The association between mid-life blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. the Honululu-Asia aging study. JAMA 1995;274:1846–51. 89 Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology 2001;56:42–8. 90 Leibson CL, Rocca WA, Hanson VA, et al. Risk of dementia among persons with diabetes mellitus: a population-based cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 1997;145:301–8. 91 Seshadri S, Beiser A, Selhub J, et al. Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 2002;346:476–83. 92 Vermeer SE, Prins ND, den Heijer T, et al. Silent brain infarcts and the risk of dementia and cognitive decline. N Engl J Med 2003;348:1215–22. 93 Fukumoto H, Rosene DL, Moss MB, et al. Beta-secretase activity increases with aging in human, monkey and mouse brain. Am J Pathol 2004;164:719–25. 94 Koo EH, Sisodia SS, Cork LC, et al. Differential expression of amyloid precursor protein mRNAs in cases of Alzheimer’s disease and in aged nonhuman primates. Neuron 1990;4:97–104. 95 Tanaka S, Liu L, Kimura J, et al. Age-related changes in the proportion of amyloid precursor protein mRNAs in Alzheimer’s disease and other neurological disorders. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 1992;15:303–10. 96 Ho L, Fukuchi K, Younkin SG. The alternatively spliced kunitz protease inhibitor domain alters amyloid beta protein precursor processing and amyloid beta protein production in cultured cells. J Biol Chem 1996;271:30929–34. 97 Saito T, Takaki Y, Iwata N, et al. Alzheimer’s disease, neuropeptides, neuropeptidase, and amyloid-beta peptide metabolism. Sci Aging Knowledge Environ 2003;3:PE1–18. 98 Farris W, Mansourian S, Chang Y, et al. Insulin-degrading enzyme regulates the levels of insulin, amyloid beta-protein, and the betaamyloid precursor protein intracellular domain in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:4162–7. 99 Funato H, Yoshimura M, Kusui K, et al. Quantitation of amyloid beta-protein (Ab) in the cortex during aging and in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Pathol 1998;152:1633–40. 100 Geula C, Wu CK, Saro D, et al. Aging renders the brain vulnerable to amyloid beta-protein neurotoxicity. Nat Med 1998;4:827–31.

ANSWERS 1. F; 2. F; 3. T; 4.F; 5. T; 6. F; 7. F; 8. T.