Cognitive reserve and its implications for ...

1 downloads 0 Views 170KB Size Report
topographic disorientation in AD, showing its effectiveness in improving route finding. In this single-case study, one patient was able to learn and retain the ...
Cogn Process DOI 10.1007/s10339-011-0410-3

REVIEW

Cognitive reserve and its implications for rehabilitation and Alzheimer’s disease Giulia Liberati • Antonino Raffone Marta Olivetti Belardinelli



Received: 27 July 2010 / Accepted: 18 May 2011 ! Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2011

Abstract According to the Cognitive reserve hypothesis, several factors related to mental engagement, such as level of education, type of occupation, leisure activities and social network, appear to affect the risk of developing clinical dementia. The present article provides an overview of the studies that have investigated the effects of mental engagement and cognitive stimulation specifically on dementia of the Alzheimer’s type (AD). Mental training and cognitive stimulation interventions in AD have been shown to be useful in increasing patients’ ability in performing activities of daily living (ADL), allowing them to maintain relative independence. Since cognitive engagement and stimulation are known to modify the brain processes to perform tasks, by recruiting alternative and more efficient networks, this review is especially focused on cognitive rehabilitation in AD patients, which has been shown to improve their global functioning and cognition. This perspective stresses the idea that cognitive reserve is not a fixed factor, but can be continuously modified by life experiences, even when the brain is already affected by neuropathology. Keywords Cognitive reserve ! Alzheimer’s disease ! Cognitive rehabilitation ! Cognitive stimulation

G. Liberati (&) ! A. Raffone ! M. Olivetti Belardinelli Interuniversity Centre for Research on Cognitive Processing in Natural and Artificial Systems (ECONA), IT, Rome, Italy e-mail: [email protected] A. Raffone ! M. Olivetti Belardinelli Department of Psychology, University ‘‘Sapienza’’ of Rome, Rome, Italy

Introduction Alzheimer’s disease (AD) can be defined as a progressive and neurodegenerative disorder, clinically manifested by cognitive deterioration, impairment of activities of daily living, and a variety of neuropsychiatric symptoms and behavioral disturbances (Cummings 2004). It has been demonstrated that some individuals have a better capacity in coping with the disease, showing a discrepancy between the extension of neuropathology and its clinical manifestations (Katzman et al. 1988). It has been hypothesized that such individual differences could be due to some kind of ‘‘cerebral reserve’’ (Stern 2003). Stern (2003) distinguished roughly between an active and a passive conception of the reserve. According to passive models, often referred to as ‘‘brain reserve models’’, individual differences in coping with neuropathology derive from structural features (e.g., brain dimension or number of synapses). According to active models or ‘‘cognitive reserve models’’, the variability in the clinical manifestation of the neuropathology reflects individuals’ different ability to use more efficient and flexible cognitive strategies, which can emerge from different life experiences. Actually, the demarcation between brain reserve and cognitive reserve cannot be so well determined, firstly because differences in cognitive processing should also have a physiologic basis, and secondly because many of the factors associated with increased cognitive reserve, such as cognitively stimulating experiences, appears to have a direct effect on brain structure (Stern 2009). Adopting a so-called ‘‘active’’ perspective, this review will focus on how engaging in mentally stimulating activities can help in preventing and coping with the clinical manifestations of AD, consistently with the cognitive reserve (CR) hypothesis. Particular emphasis will be

123

Cogn Process

placed on research about how to employ mental training and stimulation in AD rehabilitation, integrating them with standard pharmacological interventions. The article is divided into two main sections. The first section focuses on how different life features related to mental engagement, such as educational level, occupation, leisure activities and social network, could influence the onset and development of AD. The second section focuses on how to exploit mental engagement in AD rehabilitation, aiming at improving specific tasks as well as global function and cognition. Although the distinction between specific domains and global function, which is in accordance with Clare and Woods (2004), may appear blurred from a practical perspective, we decided to adopt this classification for explicative purposes, aware that the two approaches are sometimes superimposable and can often be integrated.

Life experiences, mental engagement, and Alzheimer’s disease A great number of studies have assessed how lifestyle can be related to the risk of developing dementia. Most research focused on the relation between educational attainment and AD in large-scale populations, evaluating both prevalence (Hill et al. 1993; Kokmen et al. 1993; The Canadian Study of Health and Aging 1994; De Ronchi et al. 1998; Borroni et al. 2008) and incidence (Stern et al. 1994; Fabrigoule et al. 1995; Jacobs et al. 1995; Letenneur et al. 1999; Nielsen et al. 1999; Qiu et al. 2001; Di Carlo et al. 2002; Lindsay et al. 2002) of the disease. The results coming from these studies indicate a dose–effect relation between level of education and AD, since individuals with higher levels of education show a decreased risk of developing the clinical manifestations of the neuropathology. By measuring regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF), Stern et al. (1992) found an inverse relation between education and cerebral perfusion, since patients with a higher education had the greatest flow reduction in the parieto-temporal cortex, which is a typical pattern of AD neuropathology. This study clearly shows that education cannot provide any type of immunity to the AD process, since the neuropathology occurs independently of education. However, these results also indicate that education might delay the clinical manifestations of AD, since dementia was not detected in the patients with a higher education until functioning was impaired to the same degree as that in patients with a lower education level. Stern et al. (1992) therefore proposed that AD was present in highly educated patients for a longer time, although it was not clinically evident. Consistent results were obtained in two other studies using single photon emission computed

123

tomography (SPECT, Liao et al. 2005) and fluorodeoxyglucose–positron emission tomography (FDG–PET, Perneczky et al. 2006), indicating that education can provide a reserve that compensates for neuropathological deterioration, delaying the onset of its clinical manifestations. Some studies indicate that not only reaching a high educational level, but also being employed in a highly mentally demanding occupation is related to a decreased risk of developing AD (Evans et al. 1997; Karp et al. 2004; Stern et al. 1994; Qiu et al. 2003). Anttila et al. (2002) showed that having no main lifetime occupation is also associated with an increased risk of AD. Evans et al. (1997) showed that individuals with lower socioeconomic status (SES), computed considering education, occupational prestige and income, had a higher risk of developing the disease. A study by Stern et al. (1994) indicated that the risk of developing AD was stronger in subjects with both low education and low lifetime attainment, suggesting a synergistic effect of education and occupation. Education may influence the development of AD in several ways. First of all, education may be viewed as reflecting a ‘‘brain reserve’’ (Katzman 1993), such that in subjects with greater education, increased synaptic density in the neocortex would delay the onset of AD, with a subsequent reduction in the prevalence of the pathology. The fact that subjects with a higher education may have more resistance to the progressive deterioration caused by the loss of neuronal function is also supported, counterintuitively, by the greater cortical flow reduction observed in highly educated patients (Stern et al. 1992). Assuming that the deterioration occurs independently of education, educational level may reflect cognitive capacity or stores that allow delaying the clinical manifestations of AD, which do not become evident until the deteriorations reaches a very high degree. As a result, more AD pathology is needed to cause dementia in patients with higher education. Education could also be considered as a socializing process promoting certain lifelong learning strategies, encouraging the development of decontextualized thinking and enabling the individual to perform more competently on neuropsychological tests (Gilleard 1997). According to Vance and Crowe (2006), education appears to be an important environmental experience that may enhance neural connectivity, as well as the propensity to engage in mentally stimulating activities throughout life. Finally, education may be a surrogate for an intellectual approach to life events (Friedland 1993), which can lead to a lifelong mental stimulation and an enhanced activation of the brain regions involved in memory and in cognitive processing. Considering that education and occupational complexity can influence the development of AD supports the idea that engaging in mentally stimulating activities can be beneficial for coping with neuropathology. Moreover, this idea is

Cogn Process

endorsed by animal studies (Diamond 1993; Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006), which show that complex environments can promote neuronal growth and neuroplasticity, providing a greater resilience to brain damage. Several articles show that individuals developing AD usually have few hobbies and are less involved in psychosocial activities during their lifetime (Broe et al. 1990; Crowe et al., 2003; Kondo et al. 1994; Friedland et al. 2001; Fritsch et al. 2001; Wilson et al. 2002; Verghese et al. 2003; Scarmeas et al., 2001). Friedland et al. (2001) reported a reduction of the diversity and intensity of intellectual activities during midlife in individuals with AD, compared with healthy subjects. Similarly, Wilson et al. (2002) found that subjects engaged in frequent cognitive activities such as reading, watching television or card playing were less likely to develop AD than subjects with infrequent activities. Not only the extent of premorbid cognitive experiences, but also their nature, may affect the clinical expression of AD. This is well documented in a study by Wilson et al. (2000), where premorbid reading activity, assessed by considering both reading frequency and availability of reading resources in the home environment, was shown to be positively associated with faster decline on global cognitive and verbal measures, but not on nonverbal measures. These results are consistent with the ones of Stern et al. (1992), showing that more brain degeneration is needed to cause dementia in persons with higher levels of premorbid skill. Once that AD is clinically evident, however, the protective benefits of premorbid experience are assumed to be substantially decreased, and since AD is more advanced in those with higher premorbid skill levels, they decline more rapidly. Crowe et al. (2003) claimed that one possible way that intellectual/cultural activities could enhance cognitive function in later life might be by building verbal knowledge, which could in turn promote maintenance of memory skills. Since memory problems are central in AD, activities that promote maintenance of memory could be related to clinical prevention or delay of the disease. The relation between a mentally engaging lifestyle and AD risk could be confounded by education or occupation. For instance, it is known that different active components of CR, as educational attainment and occupational complexity, are dependent on childhood intelligence (especially in modern Western societies), which is in turn affected by biological influences (Whalley et al. 2004). A higher mental ability can not only lead to better scholastic achievements and more cognitively stimulating occupations, but also to engaging in more intellectual leisure pursuits. Education, occupation, and lifestyle could all be markers of innate capacities, which might refer to both genetic background and early developmental factors

(Scarmeas and Stern 2003). Different studies have taken this confounding aspect into account. Karp et al. (2004) showed that the association between the incidence of AD and low educational attainment remained significant also when occupation-based socio-economic status was controlled for. Scarmeas et al. (2001) reported that after controlling for education and occupation, the association between leisure activities and AD risk was still present. It is possible that some subjects may have a reduction of leisure activity as a result of early, still undiagnosed disease. In this case, a lower engagement in mentally stimulating activities would be a manifestation of dementia, rather than a risk factor. Two studies have partially addressed this possibility by excluding from the analyses subjects with cognitive impairment at baseline evaluation (Scarmeas et al. 2001; Wilson et al. 2002), although the activities were recorded only a few years before dementia incidence. In both studies, subjects with high leisure activity showed a decreased risk of developing AD (respectively a 38% and a 33% reduction) compared to subjects with low leisure activity. Further studies should attempt to solve the temporality and causality issues by considering a longer interval between assessment of the activities and AD diagnosis. One aspect that is strictly related to lifestyle and mental engagement is social network. Married individuals were found to have a longer survival and a decreased risk of developing AD compared to individuals living alone or unmarried (Fratiglioni et al. 2000; Grundman et al. 1996; Ha˚kanson et al. 2009; Helmer et al. 1999; Seidler et al. 2003; Tsolaki et al. 1997; Wang et al. 2002). This is coherent with findings from the animal literature, showing that increased social interactions result in increased synaptic density and cerebral cortex thickness (Diamond 1993). The CR perspective received criticism by several authors who did not find evidence of the construct in their studies, especially for what concerns education as a protective factor for the clinical manifestations of the disease (Chandra et al. 1998; Cobb et al. 1995; De Ronchi et al. 1998; Beard et al. 1992). This incongruity may be due to methodological issues, as recruiting patients from different kinds of populations with no comparable mean educational levels. For instance, the study of Cobb et al. (1995), which did not show education to be an influent factor for AD risk, included a higher proportion of high school graduates and subjects with medium to high educational attainment. Therefore, there could possibly be a ceiling effect in the results, as well as a threshold of education, below which there is an increased AD risk. This is consistent with the results of De Ronchi et al. (1998), revealing no difference in AD prevalence between less educated and better educated, but also a higher prevalence of AD among subjects that were not educated at all compared to the ones that had

123

Cogn Process

at least a few years of schooling. On the other hand, Chandra et al. (1998) performed a community survey in rural India, where two-thirds of the individuals in the cohort were illiterate, showing no association between AD prevalence and literacy. It is important to note that in this study, overall AD prevalence was extremely low due to low life expectancy (fewer subjects living into the age of risk) and shorter survival with duration of disease. Besides, other confounding environmental risks or protective factors (e.g., related to diet or to gene–environment interaction) in this specific population could partially explain why illiteracy did not correlate with AD prevalence. Similarly, no education effect on AD risk was found in other studies on different ethnic groups, like Caribbean Hispanics and African Americans (Tang et al. 2001), and Yoruba (Ogunniyi et al. 2006), while rural living seemed to be the strongest risk factor for AD. These studies indicate that educational attainment is only one of multiple factors that affect AD risk, and in presence of environmental influences associated to poverty and to low socioeconomic status, its influence may be less evident. The numerous findings of an influence of education, occupation, leisure activity, and social network on the clinical manifestations of AD indicate that cognitive function is dynamic, rather than static (Vance and Crowe 2006). This is coherent with the ‘‘use it or lose it’’ point of view (Hultsch et al. 1999), such that cognitive processes can be modified by exercise, experience, lifestyle, and personality, as well as with the neuroplasticity model of Vance and Crowe (2006). According to this model, neuroplasticity is the potential for morphological changes in the brain to occur with the exposition to stimuli that promote learning or challenge existing neuronal connections to adapt (Vance and Crowe 2006). Subjects that are stimulated by engaging in novel and differentiated activities are also expected to experience cerebral activation, possibly producing an increase in cortex thickness that would result in CR and maintenance of cognitive ability with age. If neuroplasticity declines, the potential to stabilize or increase CR is reduced (Vance and Crowe 2006). In a following article by Vance et al. (Vance et al. 2010), cognitive status is considered to be dependent on factors that can promote either positive or negative neuroplasticity. Positive neuroplasticity refers to beneficial morphological changes in the brain (e.g., strengthening of dendritic connections) that occur with the experience of novel and changing environmental stimulation, which increase CR. Negative neuroplasticity refers to the ability of the brain to atrophy and weaken dendritic connections, decreasing CR, and is due to an unhealthy lifestyle (e.g., poor sleep hygiene, poor nutrition, substance abuse) and to a lack of novel and challenging environmental stimuli. The approach to CR is hence multifactorial: numerous positive and

123

negative factors interact simultaneously, creating a dynamic flow of cognitive functioning. Not taking into account both positive and negative factors may be one of the reasons why some studies were not successful in recruiting CR in AD patients (Chandra et al. 1998; Tang et al. 2001; Ogunniyi et al. 2006). The distinction between positive and negative neuroplasticity is consistent with the description of AD-related cerebral changes made by Teter and Ashford (2002). According to these authors, plasticity in AD may be a process of compensatory but futile modifications in the brain, which can lead to secondary neurodegenerative effects, resulting in a ‘‘plasticity burden’’. However, plasticity stimulating interventions, which aim to create neural networks that are able to replace lost function, can be clinically effective. In agreement with the model of Vance et al. (2010), we claim that factors that support neuroplasticity should be targeted for intervention. For this reason, we propose that in order to promote neuroplasticity, and therefore the maintenance of CR, cognitive activity should be exploited in AD rehabilitation.

Cognitive rehabilitation for Alzheimer’s disease The repeated finding of a relation between AD and different factors associated with cognitive engagement supports the CR hypothesis, here considered as the ability to use more efficient and flexible cognitive strategies that can be trained by means of continuous mental exercise. If it is true that a mentally stimulating lifestyle can modify the paradigms used by the brain to perform a task, then it can also be hypothesized that an intensive mental training or stimulation might provide a greater resilience in facing the neuropathology, possibly recruiting alternate networks. This perspective emphasizes that cognitive reserve is not fixed, and that at any point during life course it can result from a combination of experiences and exposures. In agreement with Vance and Crowe (2006), we believe that increasing CR can allow the maintenance of everyday functioning for longer periods of time in later life. In opposition to a widespread fatalistic attitude toward dementia, non-pharmacological interventions have been increasingly promoted in the last years, in order to optimize cognition and affect global functioning (De Vreese et al. 2001). In the following paragraphs, by using a distinction adopted by Clare and Woods (2004), we will differentiate between cognitive training of specific functions, such as memory or language, and general enhancement of cognitive and social functioning. We are aware that this distinction is problematic, since some tasks that are apparently focused on a specific function may require more global and general capacities (e.g., tasks that aim to improve

Cogn Process

orientations skills). Moreover, it is evident that general cognitive and social functioning involves the integration of single functions such as attention, language, and working memory. Nevertheless, we consider this rough distinction useful to describe the different approaches according to their declared aim (training a specific domain vs. improving global functioning). Special mention will be made of goal oriented cognitive rehabilitation applied to AD patients (Clare et al. 2010), a promising approach which focuses on the identification of personally relevant goals and on the strategies for addressing them. Cognitive training of specific functions Cognitive training involves guided practice on a set of standard tasks, which reflect specific cognitive functions. The rationale of these training methods is that regular practice has the potential to improve or maintain functioning in a given domain (Clare and Woods 2004). In order for the patients to benefit from these methods, the effects of practice should be generalized beyond the immediate training context. The ability to extend what has been learned in a specific context to other contexts has been defined as ‘‘transfer’’ (Barnett and Ceci 2002). The first cognitive training strategies for patients with AD focused mostly on memory recovery. One attempt to treat memory loss with a cognitive approach employed two training strategies derived from information processing models: a didactic training, in which imagery techniques were taught, and a problem-solving training, which presented practical solutions for everyday difficulties caused by memory loss (Zarit et al. 1982). Results showed a relatively small improvement in memory performance, limited to subjects who had received the problem-solving training. Other studies showed that improvements for recall in AD patients could be gained with some interventions such as repetition and prolonged inspection time (Heun et al. 1997a, b; Kesslak et al. 1997). In another study, where seven patients were trained with an imagery-based mnemonic on face–name retention (Ba¨ckman et al. 1991), no training gains were obtained in any of the patients but one, indicating that such method is far from being generalizable. It can be pointed out that the mnemonic used in this study drew on abilities that are usually severely impaired in dementia, and that a more effective approach could be based on less compromised abilities, such as motor action. Another study that focused on memory retraining, albeit yielding some improvements, showed that changes were not detectable in everyday life and did not influence patients’ functioning (Cahn–Weiner et al. 2003).

One study successfully used procedural memory stimulation for the rehabilitation of AD patients (Zanetti et al. 1994). The main strength of this kind of stimulation is that it relies upon procedural learning, which is part of implicit memory and is relatively well preserved in AD. It has been demonstrated that the ability of AD patients to learn and retain motor and perceptual skills even across a long retention interval is well preserved (Deweer et al. 1994). Several studies have shown that errorless learning principles can be useful for memory retraining in AD patients (Clare et al. 1999, 2000, 2001, 2002, 2003; Thivierge et al. 2008). Differently from trial and error strategy, errorless learning prevents subjects from making mistakes by encouraging them only to respond when they are sure they are correct, so that they have the opportunity to experience success at every stage of the learning process (Clare et al. 2000). Moreover, the principle behind errorless learning is that when the subject is allowed to make mistakes during the training, such errors interfere with the learning of the correct information. Eliminating mistakes and rehearsing only the correct information can facilitate learning (Anderson et al. 2001). Clare et al. (1999, 2000, 2001) applied errorless learning principles in training AD patients to learn face–name associations, showing that the ability was maintained up to one year (Clare et al. 2001). A very pragmatic application of memory training with errorless learning can be found in the study of Provencher et al. (2008), which employed the technique in coping with topographic disorientation in AD, showing its effectiveness in improving route finding. In this single-case study, one patient was able to learn and retain the procedural component of a short route in a seniors residence. However, the patient’s ability to locate rooms on a map remained impaired, showing that the route learning did not improve general orientation abilities. The fact that the route finding task did not generalize to other destinations indicates that errorless learning may only lead to a very specific route learning, which can certainly be useful, but not extendable to different circumstances (e.g., routine changes, relocation of a patient’s room). Another memory rehabilitation technique employed with AD patients is spaced retrieval (McKitrick and Camp 1993; Bird and Kinsella 1996; Camp et al. 1996), often referred to as expanding rehearsal, which facilitates new learning by exploiting implicit memory, having the subject practice recall over increasingly longer periods of time. If the retrieval is successful the preceding interval is doubled, otherwise the patient is told the right answer and is asked to repeat it. The interval is then returned to the last interval that was successful for the patient. Hence, spaced retrieval technique is based on an errorless learning approach (Anderson et al. 2001).

123

Cogn Process

Anderson et al. (2001) compared spaced retrieval with a technique based on the use of audiotapes for memory rehabilitation (Tape Therapy, Arkin 1992). Audiotapes included short segments of facts that were relevant for the patient, followed by related questions. The correct answers to the questions were provided after a brief pause, minimizing mistakes. Like spaced retrieval, tape therapy exploits a conjunction of errorless learning and implicit memory. Both spaced retrieval and tape therapy improved the subjects’ memory ability, but spaced retrieval had more rapid effects and a greater carry-over (Anderson et al. 2001). Several studies attempted to evaluate the possibility to treat both memory and related language deficits with the support of external aids (Bourgeois 1990; Bourgeois 1992; Oriani et al. 2003). Bourgeois (1990) reported that middlestage AD patients could be trained to use a prosthetic memory aid (a wallet with printed sentences, pictures, and photographs related to the patient’s life) during conversations, and the use of aids could be maintained once training stopped (Bourgeois 1992). However, changes did not seem to influence the subjects’ behavior in everyday conversation, such that partners did not report significant changes over the period of the study. Oriani et al. (2003) reported that the use of an electronic memory aid (EMA) in patients with mild-tomoderate AD improved patients’ prospective memory more than using a written list to recall. Authors underline the fact that the EMA is a memory task itself, since patients have to remember its aim, how to use it, and to carry it about. Therefore, the utility of such device may be limited to patients with a relatively mild cognitive impairment. Kawashima et al. (2005) reported that after a six-month training program named learning therapy, comprising reading aloud and arithmetic calculation, participants with a diagnosis of AD improved cognitive functions and verbal communication. According to the authors, one possible explanation of this achievement might be the improvement of executive functions during the training. General enhancement of cognitive and social functioning Several cognitive training procedures can be criticized for their lack of generalized effects in daily contexts (Zarit et al. 1982; Bourgeois 1990; Ba¨ckman et al. 1991; Bourgeois 1992; Cahn–Weiner et al. 2003), since their influence is mainly restricted to the targeted tasks and does not extend to other cognitive functions. It is however important to point out that not so many studies have evaluated the transfer of a specific skill from one context to another, and therefore further assessments are required. It has been argued that an efficient functioning in everyday life depends on the flexible use of different

123

strategies adjusted to different situations, so that single functions as memory cannot be considered as muscles that can simply be trained by means of repeated exercise (Ptak et al. 2010). In agreement with this assumption, numerous approaches aim at improving the individual’s general functioning in everyday life, without focusing on domainspecific abilities. The rationale of this choice is that cognitive functions, such as memory or communication, are not used in isolation, but are strongly integrated with other functions (e.g., attention, problem solving, etc.). One attempt to improve patients’ global functioning is the reality orientation therapy (ROT), which is a psychosocial intervention for persons that manifest memory loss, episodes of confusion, and time–place–person disorientation (Zanetti et al. 1995). ROT uses social interaction and continuous environmental stimulation, which are thought to provide a greater understanding of surrounding, possibly resulting in an improved sense of control and self-esteem (Spector et al. 2001). More specifically, caregivers and professionals are instructed to present basic orienting information during the interactions with the patients, involving them in what is happening around them and reinforcing their interest in the environment (Van der Linden et al. 2003). This therapy has been widely used with AD patients, for instance teaching them to use watches, diaries and daily behavioral itineraries to retrain verbal and behavioral orientations (Hanley and Lusty 1984; Gotestam 1987). Gerber et al. (1991) compared three groups of patients receiving respectively a reality orientation intervention, a social interaction intervention, and no intervention at all, showing that orientation and language improved significantly in the two treatment groups. Zanetti et al. (1995) evaluated the effects of a long-term group program of formal ROT, administered five days a week for one month to mild-to-moderate AD patients. Each ROT session consisted of an intensive cognitive training during which information such as date, time, current location, and participants’ names was presented. Patients could intervene with the aid of objects such as notes, calendars or clocks. Results showed that ROT decelerated cognitive decline. Other consistent findings indicate that ROT can also be applied successfully in hospital settings (Raggi et al. 2007). Similarly to reality orientation, cognitive stimulation is a procedure that involves engagement in a range of group activities and discussions, which aim at a general enhancement of cognitive and social function as opposed to focusing on specific abilities (Clare et al. 2004). Moreover, this approach emphasizes positive feedback and motivation (Quayhagen and Quayhagen 2001). Quayhagen and Quayhagen (1989) reported the efficacy of a dyadic cognitive stimulation program, in which patient and caregiver were involved in communication and problem-solving

Cogn Process

exercises. Cognitive stimulation appeared to be beneficial in numerous studies (Arkin 1992; Quayhagen et al. 1995; Arkin 1999; Arkin 2000; Quayhagen and Quayhagen 2001; Mahendra and Arkin 2003; Spector et al. 2001; Arkin 2007), also showing that involving spousal caregivers in the cognitive stimulation program (Quayhagen and Quayhagen 1989) and utilizing games (Sobel 2001) lead to significant cognitive improvements. Computer-based training has recently been introduced in the context of cognitive stimulation. In two interactive computer-based individualized cognitive programs, which used personal and biographical material related to the patient’s environment, (e.g., pictures of the patient’s usual shopping route), patients showed fewer errors, performed tasks faster and required less assistance after training (Hofmann et al. 1996; Hofmann et al. 2003). Other studies employed interactive computer-based training programs (Schreiber et al. 1998; Schreiber et al. 1999) and software for neuropsychological training [NPT], (Cipriani et al. 2006) individualized for each patient. Results showed that daily training with NPT improved performances not only at computer-based trained exercises, but also at traditional neuropsychological tests. Rehabilitation of daily living activities (e.g., brushing teeth, dressing, paying a cheque) through procedural strategies was also shown to be effective, and improvements could be evident even in daily activities that had not been trained, indicating that functional achievements may be independent of the learning context (Farina et al. 2002; Hirono et al. 1997; Zanetti et al. 1997; Zanetti et al. 2001). Another study combined errorless learning and spaced retrieval techniques for retraining patients in performing instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), such as using a voice mail or managing messages from an answering machine (Thivierge et al. 2008). Although not declaring it explicitly, these studies could be assimilated with the goaloriented cognitive rehabilitation (Clare et al. 2010), a promising approach that has only recently been adopted for the rehabilitation of AD patients. Clare et al. (2010) claim that cognitive rehabilitation should be an individualized approach, which identifies personally relevant goals and devises strategies for addressing them. The goal-oriented cognitive rehabilitation focuses on reducing functional disability and increasing activity and social participation. In their study, Clare et al. (2010) identified for each participant five personally relevant goals in areas relating to self-care, leisure, and productivity. Examples of therapy goals included remembering activities to do in the house, the use of a mobile phone, and maintaining concentration while cooking. The rehabilitation consisted of eight weekly 1-hour individual sessions, which comprised learning practical aids and strategies, techniques for learning new information, attention and concentration exercises, and

techniques for stress management. Participants were encouraged to work on their goals by practicing the techniques between sessions. Caregivers were also involved to support between-session practice. Descriptive statistics indicated that cognitive goal-oriented rehabilitation improved patients’ performance, and the improvement was more evident when a caregiver was available for support. The authors point out that the preliminary positive outcomes of this rehabilitation approach were obtained with a very brief intervention, and that longer and more intensive interventions may produce stronger effects in terms of goal attainment. Research should therefore proceed in the direction of goal-oriented trainings, adopting quantitative reliable measures. Special mention should also be given to music therapy with AD patients, which is reported to improve significantly the quality of life of patients and caregivers (Koger et al. 1999). In fact, patterns of music cognition may differ from abilities in other domains, and caregivers often describe preserved musical functioning in their relatives or clients with AD (Vanstone and Cuddy 2009). Smith–Marchese (1994) reported an amelioration of cognitive deficits in AD patients that participated for six weeks in musical events, which comprised playing simple musical instruments, singing, and body movement. Thompson et al. (2005) reported that exposure to classical music increased cognitive performance in AD patients, leading to better performances on category fluency tasks. Aldridge (1995) underlined that music therapy can enhance socialization and communication skills, leading to a greater cognitive stimulation of the patient. Several case studies describe AD patients who actively engage in productive musical activities such as playing musical instruments (Beatty et al. 1988, 1994; Fornazzari et al. 2006). Cuddy and Duffin (2005) reported the case of a patient with severe AD who had preserved recognition memory for familiar melodies. Engaging in musical activities, such as listening to music or playing an instrument, can be one way to enhance cognitive reserve, since music training is known to affect cortex morphology and organization (Schlaug 2001; Ohnishi et al. 2001). Moreover, when music faculties are spared in the presence of AD, enjoyment, and relaxation conveyed by the familiarity with music (Vanstone et al. 2009) can be exploited in rehabilitation.

Discussion According to the CR theory (Stern 2002, 2003), individual differences in coping with neuropathology can emerge from different life experiences, such as educational attainment, occupation complexity, mentally stimulating leisure activities, and social network. The present review focused on how mental engagement can influence the

123

Cogn Process

clinical manifestations of AD and how this can be exploited in rehabilitation. The analysis of the literature supports the idea that lifestyle can provide a reserve that allows better coping with AD neuropathology. Education and occupation have been associated with the risk of AD in several population studies, and, for equal clinical conditions, the extent of neuropathology in the brain is greater in more educated individuals, meaning that education can provide a reserve that allows coping better with the disease. Together with these findings, it has been shown that different aspects of lifestyle, such as leisure activities and social network, are predictors of a greater resilience to Alzheimer neuropathology. Considering cognitive reserve as the ability to utilize more efficient and flexible cognitive strategies, which can be trained by means of a continuous mental exercise, it makes sense to consider the possibility of exploiting this capacity in the context of AD rehabilitation. The idea is that CR is not a fixed factor, but is rather continuously modified by environment and life experiences through the whole life course. If it is true that education, occupation, and lifestyle can modify the brain processes to perform a task, then it can also be hypothesized that an intensive mental training or stimulation could provide a greater resilience in facing the neuropathology, possibly recruiting alternate networks. Improving cognitive and functional abilities appears to be a realistic goal in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. According to several authors, it is preferable for rehabilitation to focus on enhancing global cognitive function rather than training distinct domains, since in everyday life, memory, language, executive functions, and reasoning are closely related. Focusing on global functioning would also allow extending the effects of the training to everyday life. It is important to note, however, that since there is still a lack of studies assessing the possibility in AD patients of transferring a specific ability from one context to another, more research in this direction is required before drawing a definite conclusion. Another aspect that needs to be considered is that while it is usually feasible to assess patients’ improvements in a specific domain by using adequate neuropsychological instruments, evaluating global functioning in a standardized way is much more complex. Therefore, both approaches could be usefully attempted, as long as the main objective remains the improvement of the patient’s everyday life. A very positive aspect of Alzheimer rehabilitation is the actual focus on activities of daily living (ADL), which aims at increasing the length of time patients are able to remain relatively independent, hence limiting caregivers’ burden. It is therefore extremely important to design rehabilitation programs that consider everyday problems that are revealed by patients and caregivers, in order to improve the patients’ quality of life. A detailed planning of the

123

intervention is fundamental for gaining results. Ptak et al. (2010) highlighted the importance of defining target problems and of focusing rehabilitation on concrete goals that should be specific for each subject. This is in agreement with the cognitive goal-oriented rehabilitation approach, which is based on the definition of specific techniques and strategies that are personalized on the basis of the patient’s specific goals and abilities, encouraging the support of caregivers. The involvement of caregivers should be taken into account, not only in the definition of the intervention, but also for supporting goal achievement and in order to have a feedback of the effectiveness of the intervention. The encouraging findings of Clare et al. (2010) regarding the use of cognitive goal-oriented rehabilitation with AD patient should lead to further investigations in this direction. Being aware of the impact of a lifelong mental stimulation on neuroplasticity and on coping with neuropathology in older age should lead to a greater investment on cognitive interventions for the rehabilitation of the elderly. So far, the question whether there is a difference in the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with different years of schooling, different lifelong occupations, and different leisure activities has not been assessed directly. Only in one study (Olazara´n et al. 2004), which focused on the benefits of a cognitive-motor stimulation program on patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and mildto-moderate AD, cognitive response appeared to be higher in individuals with lower education. The authors interpreted this counterintuitive result within the CR framework, according to which, given a similar level of clinical severity, AD pathology would be more advanced in patients with higher education, rendering them in an inferior learning potential condition. Conducting more studies in this direction would be of great importance, allowing the planning of different rehabilitation strategies by taking into account the patients’ different life experiences. What we can learn from research on CR is the relevance of those particular contexts that facilitate mental engagement, personal growth, and socialization, in promoting more flexible cognitive strategies and therefore more resistance to neuropathology. For this reason, it is advisable to perform rehabilitation within naturalistic settings, where patients have the possibility to spontaneously engage in different activities and socialize with other individuals. A very good example of such context can be found in Adam et al. (2010), describing the management of a patient in a day care center that reproduced a real-life environment, not only aiming at optimizing autonomy and ADL, but also promoting socialization and leisure-oriented activities. An important aspect that should not be disregarded in the rehabilitation process is the patient’s emotional response. For instance, Kazui et al. (2003) underlined that

Cogn Process

AD patients can show a memory advantage for emotional stimuli just like healthy subjects. Musical training can certainly be a source of intense emotional experiences, which can enhance motivation. Vanstone and Cuddy (2009) suggested that music could have powerful therapeutic effects if its emotional properties were considered in planning interventions, for instance developing musical cues that could enhance memory by inducing emotions, consequently increasing familiarity with specific aspects of the patient’s daily routine. The positive effects of music on memory are evident in a recent study by Simmons–Stern et al. (2010), showing that sung lyrics were remembered by AD patients better than spoken lyrics. So far, most rehabilitation interventions have been conceived for patients with mild or moderate AD. A future challenge would be to focus on further stages of the disease. Since in the latest stages of AD communication can be extremely difficult (Miller 1989; Au et al. 1988) interaction should be based on channels other than speech (e.g., gestures, emotional expression). A first attempt in this direction has been recently made with ‘‘passive’’ or ‘‘affective’’ brain–computer interfaces (Nijboer et al. 2009), which aim at extracting involuntary brain signals and utilizing them for human–computer interaction. Adapting affective brain–computer interfaces to Alzheimer patients would allow the restoration of some form of communication even in the latest stages of the disease, leading in the future to new paths for cognitive rehabilitation.

References Adam S, Van der Linden M, Juillerat A-C, Salmon E (2010) The cognitive management of daily life activities in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease in day care centre: a case report. Neuropsychol Rehabil 10:485–509 Aldridge D (1995) Music therapy and the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Gerontol 16(1):41–57 Anderson J, Arens K, Johnson R, Coppens P (2001) Spaced retrieval vs. memory tape therapy in memory rehabilitation for dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Clin Gerontol 24(1):123–140 Anttila T, Helkala EL, Kivipelto M, Hallikainen M, Alhainen K, Heinonen H, Mannermaa A, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A (2002) Midlife income, occupation, APOE status, and dementia: a population-based study. Neurology 59(6):887–893 Arkin SM (1992) Audio-assisted memory training with early Alzheimer’s patients: two single subject experiments. Clin Gerontol 12(2):77–96 Arkin SM (1999) Elder rehab: a student-supervised exercise program for Alzheimer’s patients. The Gerontologist 39(6):729–735 Arkin SM (2000) Alzheimer memory training: students replicate learning successes. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 15(3):152–162 Arkin S (2007) Language-enriched exercise plus socialization slows cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 22(1):62–77

Au R, Albert ML, Obler LK (1988) The relationship of aphasia to dementia. Aphasiology 2:161–173 Ba¨ckman L, Josephsson S, Herlitz A, Stigsdotter A, Viitanen M (1991) The generalizability of training gains in dementia: effects of an imagery-based mnemonic on face-name retention duration. Psychol Aging 6(3):489–492 Barnett S, Ceci S (2002) When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for Far transfer. Psychol Bull 128:612–637 Beard CM, Kokmen E, Offord P, Kurland LT (1992) Lack of association between Alzheimer’s disease and education, occupation, marital status, or living arrangement. Neurology 42(11): 2063 Beatty WW, Zavadil KD, Bailly RC, Rixen GJ, Farnham N, Fisher L (1988) Preserved musical skill in a severely demented patient. Int J Clin Neuropsychol 10:158–164 Beatty WW, Winn P, Adams RL, Allen EW, Wilson DA, Prince JR, Olson KA, Dean K, Littleford D (1994) Preserved cognitive skills in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 51:1040–1046 Bird M, Kinsella G (1996) Long-term cued recall of tasks in senile dementia. Psychol Aging 11(1):45–56 Borroni B, Alberici A, Agosti C, Premi E, Padovani A (2008) Education plays a different role in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23(8):796–800 Bourgeois MS (1990) Enhancing conversation skills in patients with Alzheimer’s disease using a prosthetic memory aid. J Appl Behav Anal 23(1):29–42 Bourgeois MS (1992) Evaluating memory wallets in conversations with persons with dementia. J Speech Hear Disord 35(6): 1344–1357 Broe GA, Henderson AS, Creasey H et al (1990) A case–control study of Alzheimer’s disease in Australia. Neurology 40:1698–1707 Cahn-Weiner DA, Malloy PF, Rebok GW, Ott BR (2003) Results of a randomized placebo-controlled study of memory training for mildly impaired Alzheimer’s disease patients. Appl Neuropsychol 10(4):215–223 Camp CJ, Foss JW, O’Hanlon AM, Stevens AB (1996) Memory interventions for persons with dementia. Appl Cogn Psychol 10(3):193–210 Chandra V, Ganguli M, Pandav R, Johnston J, Belle S, DeKosky ST (1998) Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias in rural India: the Indo-US study. Neurology 51(4):1000–1008 Cipriani G, Bianchetti A, Trabucchi M (2006) Outcomes of a computer-based cognitive rehabilitation program on Alzheimer’s disease patients compared with those on patients affected by mild cognitive impairment. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 43(3): 327–335 Clare L, Woods RT (2004) Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Neuropsychol Rehabil 14(4):385–401 Clare L, Wilson BA, Breen K, Hodges JR (1999) Errorless learning of face-name associations in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocase 5(1):37–46 Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Breen K, Gosses A, Hodges JR (2000) Intervening with everyday memory problems in dementia of Alzheimer type: an errorless learning approach. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 22(1):132–146 Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Hodges JR, Adams M (2001) Longterm maintenance of treatment gains following a cognitive rehabilitation intervention in early dementia of Alzheimer type: a single case study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 11(3):477–494 Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Roth I, Hodges JR (2002) Relearning face-name associations in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 16(4):538–547 Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Hodges JR (2003) Cognitive rehabilitation as a component of early intervention in

123

Cogn Process Alzheimer’s disease: a single case study. Aging Ment Health 7(1):15–21 Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Roth I, Hodges JR (2004) Awareness in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to outcome of cognitive rehabilitation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26(2):215–226 Clare L, Linden D, Woods R, Whitaker R, Evans S, Parkinson C, van Paasschen J, Nelis S, Hoare Z, Yuen K, Rugg M (2010) Goaloriented cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage alzheimer disease: a single-blind randomized controlled trial of clinical efficacy. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 18:928–939 Cobb JL, Wolf PA, Au R, White R (1995) The effect of education on the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in the Framingham study. Neurology 45(9):1707–1712 Crowe M, Andel R, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, Gatz M (2003) Does participation in leisure activities lead to reduced risk of Alzheimer’s disease? A prospective study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol B-Psychol 58(5):P249–P255 Cuddy LL, Duffin JM (2005) Music, memory, and Alzheimer’s disease: is music recognition spared in dementia, and how can it be assessed? Med Hypotheses 64:229–235 Cummings JL (2004) Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 351:56–67 De Ronchi D, Fratiglioni L, Rucci P, Paternico` A, Graziani S, Dalmonte E (1998) The effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with middle to high socioeconomic status. Neurology 50(5):1231–1238 De Vreese LP, Neri M, Fioravanti M, Belloi L, Zanetti O (2001) Memory rehabilitation in Alzheimer’s disease: a review of progress. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 16:794–809 Deweer B, Ergis AM, Fossati P, Pillon B (1994) Explicit memory, procedural learning and lexical priming in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 30(1):113–126 Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci LA et al (2002) Incidence of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia in Italy. The ILSA study. J Am Geriatr Soc 50:41–48 Diamond M (1993) An optimistic view of the aging brain. Generations 17:31–33 Evans DA, Hebert LE, Beckett LA, Scherr PA, Albert MS, Chown MJ, Pilgrim DM, Taylor JO (1997) Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Arch Neurol 54(11):1399–1405 Fabrigoule C, Letenneur L, JFo Dartigues, Zarrouk M (1995) Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc 43(5):485–490 Farina E, Fioravanti R, Chiavari L, Imbornone E, Alberoni M, Pomati S, Pinardi G, Pegnatti R, Mariani C (2002) Comparing two programs of cognitive training in Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. Acta Neurol Scand 105(5):365–371 Fornazzari L, Castle T, Nadkarni S, Ambrose M, Miranda D, Apanasiewicz N et al (2006) Preservation for episodic musical memory in a pianist with Alzheimer disease. Neurology 66:610–611 Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B (2000) Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 355(9212): 1315–1319 Friedland RP (1993) Epidemiology, education, and the ecology of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43(2):246–249 Friedland RP, Fritsch T, Smyth KA, Koss E, Lerner AJ, Chen CH, Petot GJ, Debanne SM (2001) Patients with Alzheimer’s disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98(6): 3440–3445 Fritsch T, McClendon MJ, Smyth KA, Lerner AJ, Chen CH, Petot GJ, Debanne SM, Soas A, Friedland RP (2001) Effects of educational attainment on the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s

123

disease: results from a research registry. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 16(6):369–376 Gerber GJ, Prince PN, Snider HG, Atchison K, Dubois L, Kilgour JA (1991) Group activity and cognitive improvement among patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Hosp Community Psychiatry 42(8):843–845 Gilleard CJ (1997) Education and Alzheimer’s disease: a review of recent international epidemiological studies. Aging Ment Health 1(1):33–46 Gotestam KG (1987) Learning versus environmental support for increasing reality orientation in senile demented patients. Eur J Psychiatry 1:7–12 Grundman M, Ellis R, Olichney JM et al (1996) Importance of clinical features and marital status on survival in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 17:S88–S89 Ha˚kanson K, Rovio S, Helkala E-L, Vilska A-R, Winblad B, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Mohammed AH, Kivipelto M (2009) Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: population based cohort study. Br Med J 339(7712):1–12 Hanley IG, Lusty K (1984) Case histories and shorter communications: memory aids in reality orientation: a single-case study. Behav Res Ther 22(6):709–712 Helmer C, Damon D, Letenneur L, Fabrigoule C, Barberger-Gateau P, Lafont S, Fuhrer R, Antonucci T, Commenges D, Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF (1999) Marital status and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a French population-based cohort study. Neurology 53(9):1953–1958 Heun R, Burkart M, Benkert O (1997a) Effect of repetition and inspection times on picture recall in patients with dementia of Alzheimer type. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 8(3):152–156 Heun R, Burkart M, Benkert O (1997b) Improvement of a picture recall by repetition in patients with dementia of Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12(1):85–92 Hill LR, Klauber MR, Salmon DP, Yu ES (1993) Functional status, education, and the diagnosis of dementia in the Shanghai survey. Neurology 43(1):138–145 Hirono N, Mori E, Ikejiri Y, Imamura T, Shimomura T, Ikeda M, Yamashita H, Takatsuki Y, Tokimasa A, Yamadori A (1997) Procedural memory in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 8(4):210–216 Hofmann M, Hock C, Ku¨hler A, Mu¨ller-Spahn F (1996) Interactive computer-based cognitive training in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Psychiatr Res 30(6):493–501 Hofmann M, Ro¨sler A, Schwarz W, Mu¨ller-Spahn F, Krauchi K, Hock C, Seifritz E (2003) Interactive computer-training as a therapeutic tool in Alzheimer’s disease. Compr Psychiatry 44(3):213–219 Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA (1999) Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 14(2):245–263 Jacobs DM, Sano M, Dooneief G, Marder K (1995) Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 45(5):957–962 Karp A, Kareholt I, Qiu C, Bellander T, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2004) Relation of education and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Epidemiol 159(2):175–183 Katzman R (1993) Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43(1):13–20 Katzman R, Brown T, Thal LJ, Fuld PA, Aronson M, Butters N, Klauber MR, Wiederholt W, Pay M, Xiong RB (1988) Comparison of rate of annual change of mental status score in four independent studies of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 24(3):384–389 Kawashima R, Okita K, Yamazaki R, Tajima N, Yoshida H, Taira M, Iwata K, Sasaki T, Maeyama K, Usui N, Sugimoto K (2005)

Cogn Process Reading aloud and arithmetic calculation improve frontal function of people with dementia. J Gerontol A-Biol 60(3): 380–384 Kazui H, Mori E, Hashimoto M, Hirono N (2003) Enhancement of declarative memory by emotional arousal and visual memory function in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 15:221–226 Kesslak JP, Nackoul K, Sandman CA (1997) Memory training for individuals with Alzheimer’s disease improves name recall. Behav Neurol 10(4):137–142 Koger SM, Chapin K, Brotons M (1999) Is music therapy an effective intervention for dementia? A meta-analytic review of the literature. J Music Ther 36:2–15 Kokmen E, Beard CM, O’Brien J, Kurland LT (1993) Educational attainment and Alzheimer’s disease: reassessment of the Rochester, Minnesota data (1975–1984). Neurology 43:A317 Kondo K, Niino M, Shido K (1994) A case-control study of Alzheimer’s disease in Japan: significance of life-styles. Dementia 5(6):314–326 Letenneur L, Gilleron V, Commenges D, Helmer C, Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF (1999) Are sex and educational level independent predictors of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease? Incidence data from the PAQUID project. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 66(2):177–183 Liao Y-C, Liu R-S, Teng EL, Lee Y-C, Wang P-N, Lin K-N, Chung C-P, Liu H-C (2005) Cognitive reserve: a SPECT study of 132 Alzheimer’s disease patients with an education range of 0–19 years. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 20(1):8–14 Lindsay J, Laurin D, Verreault R, He´bert R, Helliwell B, Hill GB, McDowell I (2002) Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective analysis from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Am J Epidemiol 156(5):445–453 Mahendra N, Arkin S (2003) Effects of four years of exercise, language, and social interventions on Alzheimer discourse. J Comm Disord 36(5):395–422 McKitrick LA, Camp CJ (1993) Relearning the names of things: the spaced-retrieval intervention implemented by a caregiver. Clin Gerontol 14(2):60–62 Miller E (1989) Language impairment in Alzheimer type dementia. Clin Psychol Rev 9(2):181–195 Nielsen H, Lolk A, Andersen K, Andersen J, Kragh-Sørensen P (1999) Charateristics of elderly who develop Alzheimer’s disease during the next two years—a neuropsychological study using CAMCOG: the Odense Study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14(11):957–963 Nijboer F, Hoffmann U, Morin F, Carmien S, Leon E, Koene R (2009) Affective brain-computer interfaces: psychophysiological markers of emotion in healthy people and people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. In: Workshop on affective braincomputer interfaces & IEEE international workshop on social signal processing ABCI 2009, 10–12 Sept 2009, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Proceedings vol II, pp 1–11 Nithianantharajah J, Hannan A (2006) Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:697–709 Ogunniyi A, Hall KS, Gureje O, Baiyewu O, Gao S, Unverzagt FW, Smith-Gamble V, Evans RE, Dickens J, Musick BS, Hendrie HC (2006) Risk factors for incident Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans and Yoruba. Metab Brain Dis 21(2–3):235–240 Ohnishi T, Matsuda H, Asada T, Aruga M, Hirakata M, Nishikawa M et al (2001) Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cereb Cortex 11:754–760 Olazara´n J, Mun˜iz R, Reisberg B, Pen˜a-Casanova J, del Ser T, CruzJentoft AJ, Serrano P, Navarro E, Garcia de la Rocha ML, Frank A, Galiano M, Ferna´ndez-Bullido Y, Serra JA, Gonza´lesSalvador MT, Servilla C (2004) Benefits of cognitive-motor

intervention in MCI and mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Neurology 63:2348–2353 Oriani M, Moniz-Cook E, Binetti G, Zanieri G, Frisoni GB, Geroldi C, De Vreese LP, Zanetti O (2003) An electronic memory aid to support prospective memory in patients in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. Aging Ment Health 7(1): 22–27 Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Diehl-Schmid J, Schmid G, Wohlschla¨ger A, Kars S, Grimmer T, Wagenpfeil S, Monsch A, Kurz A (2006) Schooling mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer’s disease: findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose-positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77(9):1060–1063 Provencher Vr, Bier N, Trs Audet, Gagnon L (2008) Errorless-based techniques can improve route finding in early Alzheimer’s disease: a case study. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 23(1):47–56 Ptak R, Van der Linden M, Schnider A (2010) Cognitive rehabilitation of episodic memory disorders: from theory to practice. Front Hum Neurosci 4(57):1–11 Qiu C, Ba¨ckman L, Winblad B, Agu¨ero-Torres H, Fratiglioni L (2001) The influence of education on clinically diagnosed dementia incidence and mortality data from the Kungsholmen Project. Arch Neurol 58(12):2034–2039 Qiu C, Karp A, von Strauss E, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L, Bellander T (2003) Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in the Kungsholmen project. Am J Ind Med 43(2):204–211 Quayhagen MP, Quayhagen M (1989) Differential effects of familybased strategies on Alzheimer’s disease. Gerontologist 29(2):150–155 Quayhagen MP, Quayhagen M (2001) Testing of cognitive stimulation intervention for dementia caregiving dyads. Neuropsychol Rehabil 11(3/4):319–332 Quayhagen MP, Quayhagen M, Corbeil RR, Roth PA, Rodgers JA (1995) A dyadic remediation program for care recipients with dementia. Nurs Res 44:153–159 Raggi A, Iannaccone S, Marcone A, Ginex V, Ortelli P, Nonis A, Giusti MC, Cappa SF (2007) The effects of a comprehensive rehabilitation program of Alzheimer’s disease in a hospital setting. Behav Neurol 18(1):1–6 Scarmeas N, Stern Y (2003) Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25(5):625–633 Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern Y (2001) Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 57(12):2236–2242 Schlaug G (2001) The brain of musicians. A model for functional and structural adaptation. Ann NY Acad Sci 930:281–299 Schreiber M, Lutz K, Schweizer A, Kalveram KT, Ja¨ncke L (1998) Development and evaluation of an interactive computer-based training as a rehabilitation tool for dementia. Psychol Beitr 40(1):85–102 Schreiber M, Schweizer A, Lutz K, Kalveram KT, Ja¨ncke L (1999) Potential of an interactive computer-based training in the rehabilitation of dementia: an initial study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 9(2):155–167 Seidler A, Bernhardt T, Nienhaus A, Fro¨lich L (2003) Association between the psychosocial network and dementia—a case-control study. J Psychiat Res 37(2):89–98 Simmons-Stern NR, Budson AE, Ally BA (2010) Music as a memory enhancer in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 48(10):3164–3167 Smith-Marchese K (1994) The effects of participatory music on the reality orientation and sociability of Alzheimer’s residents in a long-term care setting. Activ Adapt Aging 18(2):41–55 Sobel BP (2001) Bingo vs. physical intervention in stimulating shortterm cognition in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 16(2):115–120

123

Cogn Process Spector A, Orrell M, Davies S, Woods B (2001) Can reality orientation be rehabilitated? Development and piloting of an evidence-based programme of cognition-based therapies for people with dementia. Neuropsychol Rehabil 11(3):377–397 Stern Y (2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and reserve application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8:448–460 Stern Y (2003) The concept of cognitive reserve: a catalyst for research. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25:589–593 Stern Y (2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47(10): 2015–2028 Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, Mayeux R (1992) Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 32(3):371–375 Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX (1994) Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 271(13):1004–1010 Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, Jacobs DM, Small S, Bell K, Merchant C, Lantigua R, Costa R, Stern Y, Mayeux R (2001) Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology 56:49–56 Teter B, Ashford W (2002) Neuroplasticity in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci Res 70:402–437 The Canadian Study of Health and Aging (1994) Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease in Canada. Neurology 44(11):2073–2080 Thivierge S, Simard M, Jean L, Grandmaison E´ (2008) Errorless learning and spaced retrieval techniques to relearn instrumental activities of daily living in mild Alzheimer’s disease: a case report study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 4(5):987–999 Thompson RG, Moulin CJA, Hayre S, Jones RW (2005) Music enhances category fluency in healthy older adults and Alzheimer’s disease patients. Exp Aging Res 31(1):91–99 Tsolaki M, Fountoulakis K, Chantzi E, Kazis A (1997) Risk factors for clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease: a case-control study of a Greek population. Int Psychogeriatr 9(3):327–341 Van der Linden M, Juillerat A-C, Adam S (2003) Cognitive intervention. In: Mulligan R, Van der Linden M, Juillerat A-C (eds) The clinical management of early Alzheimer’s disease: a handbook. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp 169–233 Vance DE, Crowe M (2006) A proposed model of neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve in older adults. Activ Adapt Aging 30(3):61–79 Vance DE, Roberson AJ, McGuinness TM, Fazeli PL (2010) How neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve protect cognitive functioning. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv 48(4):23–30

123

Vanstone AD, Cuddy LL (2009) Musical memory in Alzheimer disease. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 17(1):108–128 Vanstone AD, Cuddy LL, Duffin JM, Alexander E (2009) Exceptional preservation of memory for tunes and lyrics. Ann NY Acad Sci 1169:291–294 Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, Ambrose AF, Sliwinski M, Buschke H (2003) Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 348(25):2508–2516 Wang H-X, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2002) Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. Am J Epidemiol 155(12):1081–1087 Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL, Starr JM (2004) Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Res Rev 3:369–382 Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Gilley DW, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Evans DA (2000) Premorbid reading activity and patterns of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 57(12):1718–1723 Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Aggarwal NT, Mendes De Leon CF, Morris MC, Schneider JA, Evans DA (2002) Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology 59(12):1910–1914 Zanetti O, Magni E, Binetti G, Bianchetti A (1994) Is procedural memory stimulation effective in Alzheimer’s disease? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 9(12):1006–1007 Zanetti O, Frisoni GB, De Leo D, Dello Buono M, Bianchetti A, Trabucchi M (1995) Reality orientation therapy in Alzheimer disease: useful or not? A controlled study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Dis 9(3):132–138 Zanetti O, Binetti G, Magni E, Rozzini L, Bianchetti A, Trabucchi M (1997) Procedural memory stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease: impact of a training programme. Acta Neurol Scand 95(3):152–157 Zanetti O, Zanieri G, Di Giovanni G, De Vreese LP, Pezzini A, Metitieri T, Trabucchi M (2001) Effectiveness of procedural memory stimulation in mild Alzheimer’s disease patients: a controlled study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 11(3):263–272 Zarit SH, Zarit JM, Reever KE (1982) Memory training for severe memory loss: effects on senile dementia patients and their families. The Gerontologist 22(4):373–377