Modeling Route Choice Behavior Using Stochastic Learning Automata

15 downloads 66 Views 436KB Size Report
Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey. Piscataway, NJ 08854-8014 ... 3. This paper attempts to introduce a new route choice model by modeling route ...
Paper No. 01-0372

Duplication for publication or sale is strictly prohibited without prior written permission of the Transportation Research Board

Title: Modeling Route Choice Behavior Using Stochastic Learning Automata

Authors: Kaan Ozbay, Aleek Datta, Pushkin Kachroo

Transportation Research Board 80th Annual Meeting January 7-11, 2001 Washington, D.C.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

1

MODELING ROUTE CHOICE BEHAVIOR USING STOCHASTIC LEARNING AUTOMATA By

Kaan Ozbay, Assistant Professor Department of Civil & Environmental Engineering Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Piscataway, NJ 08854-8014 Aleek Datta, Research Assistant Department of Civil & Environmental Engineering Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Piscataway, NJ 08854-8014 Pushkin Kachroo, Assistant Professor Bradley Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Blacksburg, VA 24061

ABSTRACT This paper analyzes day-to-day route choice behavior of drivers by introducing a new route choice model developed using stochastic learning automata (SLA) theory. This dayto-day route choice model addresses the learning behavior of travelers based on experienced travel time and day-to-day learning. In order to calibrate the penalties of the model, an Internet based Route Choice Simulator (IRCS) was developed. The IRCS is a traffic simulation model that represents within day and day-to-day fluctuations in traffic and was developed using Java programming. The calibrated SLA model is then applied to a simple transportation network to test if global user equilibrium, instantaneous equilibrium, and driver learning have occurred over a period of time. It is observed that the developed stochastic learning model accurately depicts the day-to-day learning behavior of travelers. Finally, it is shown that the sample network converges to equilibrium, both in terms of global user and instantaneous equilibrium. Key Words: Route Choice Behavior, Travel Simulator, Java

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

2

1.0 INTRODUCTION & MOTIVATION In recent years, engineers have foreseen the use of advanced traveler information systems (ATIS), specifically route guidance, as a means to reduce traffic congestion. Unfortunately, experimental data that reflects traveler response to route guidance is not available in abundance. Especially, effective models of route choice behavior that capture the day-to-day learning behavior of drivers are needed to estimate traveler response to information, to engineer ATIS systems, and to evaluate them as well. Extensive data is required for developing the route choice models necessary to provide understanding of traveler response to traffic conditions. Route guidance by providing traffic information can be a useful solution only if drivers’ route choice and learning behavior is well understood.

As a result of these developments and needs in the area of ATIS, several researchers have been recently working on the development of realistic route choice models with or without a learning component. Most of the previous work uses the concept of "discrete choice models" for modeling user choice behavior. The discrete choice modeling approach is a very useful one except for the fact that it requires accurate and quite large data sets for the estimation and calibration of driver utility function. Moreover, there is no direct proof that drivers make day-to-day route choice decisions based on the utility functions. Due to the recognition of these and other difficulties in developing, calibrating, and subsequently justifying the use of the existing approaches for route choice, we propose the use of an "artificial intelligence" type of approach, namely stochastic learning automata (SLA) (Narendra 1988).

The concept of learning automaton grew from a fusion of the work of psychologists in modeling observed behavior, the efforts of statisticians to model the choice of experiments based on past observations, and the efforts of system engineers to make random control and optimization decisions in random environments.

In the case of "route choice behavior modeling", which also occurs in a stochastic environment, stochastic learning automata mimics the day-to-day learning of drivers by updating the route choice probabilities based on the basis of information received and the experience of drivers. Of course the appropriate selection of the proper learning algorithm as well as the parameters it contains is crucial for its success in modeling user route choice behavior.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

3

This paper attempts to introduce a new route choice model by modeling route choice behavior using stochastic learning automata theory that is widely used in biological and engineering systems. This route choice model addresses the learning behavior of travelers based on experienced travel times (day-to-day learning). In simple terms, the stochastic learning automata approach adopted in this paper is an inductive inference mechanism which updates the probabilities of its actions occurring in a stochastic environment in order to improve a certain performance index, i.e. travel time of users.

2.0 LITERATURE REVIEW According to the underlying hypothesis of discrete choice models, an individual’s preferences for each alternative when faced with a set of choices (routes or modes) can be represented by using a utility measure.

This utility measure is assumed to be a function of the attributes of the

alternatives as well as the decision maker’s characteristics. The decision maker is assumed to choose the alternative with the highest utility. The utility of each alternative to a specific decision maker can be expressed as a function of observed attributes of the available alternatives and the relevant observed characteristics of the decision maker (Sheffi, 1985). Now, let a denote the vector of variables which includes these attributes and characteristics, and let the utility function be denoted as Vk (ak)= Vk = Utility Function .

The distribution of the utilities is a function of this attribute vector a. Therefore the probability that alternative k will be chosen, Pk can be related to a using the widely used “multinomial logit” (MNL) choice model and of the following form (Sheffi, 1985):

Pk =

e Vk

∀ k ∈Κ

Κ

∑e

(1)

Vl

l =1

Pk = Pk (ak) = Choice Function

Pk has all the properties of an element of a probability mass function, that is,

0 ≤ Pk (ak)≤1 Κ

∑ P (ak) = 1 k=1

k

∀ k ∈Κ

(2)

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

4

In a recent paper by Vaughn et al. (1996), a general multinomial choice for route choice under ATIS is given. The utility function V k can be expressed in the functional form: V = f(Individual characteristics, Route specific characteristics, Expectations, Information, Habit persistence) The variables in each group are (Vaughn et al., 1996): • Individual characteristics: gender, age level, and education level. • Route specific attributes: number of stop locations on route. • Expectations: expected travel time, standard deviation of the expected travel time, expected stop time. • Information: incident information, congestion information, pre-trip information. • Habit: Habit persistence, or habit strength. For the base model, a simple utility function of the following form is used:

Vijt = a i + ß(E(tt ijt ))

(3)

where,

Vijt = expected utility of route j on day t for individual i E(ttijt) = expected travel time on route j on day t for individual i

α j = route specific constant

Several computer-based experiments have recently been conducted to produce route choice models. Iida, Akiyama, and Uchida (1992) developed a route choice model based on actual and predicted travel times of drivers. Their model of route choice behavior is based upon a driver’s traveling experience. During the course of the experiment, each driver was asked to predict travel times for the route to be taken (1 O-D pair only), and after the route was traveled, the actual travel time was displayed. Based on this knowledge, the driver was asked to repeat this procedure for several iterations. However, the travel time on the alternative route was not given to the driver, and the driver was not allowed to keep records of past travel times. In a second experiment, the driver was asked to perform the same tasks, but was allowed to keep records of the travel times. Two models were developed, one for each experiment. The actual and predicted travel times for route r for the i-th participant and the n-th iteration are defined as t rin , tˆrin , respectively, and the predicted travel time on the route r for the n + 1-th iteration, t rin + 1 , is corrected based on the n-th iterations actual

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

5

travel time and its deviation from the predicted travel time for that iteration. The travel time prediction model for the second case is as follows:

y = α + β1 x0 + β2 x1 + β3 x3

(4)

xk = tˆrin − k - t rin − k , y = tˆrin + 1 − t rin

(5)

where,

The parameters β1,β2,and β3 are regression coefficients. X is the difference between the actual and predicted travel times of the i-th participant for the n-th iteration, and y represents the adjusting factor for the n+1-th iteration’s predicted travel time from the n-th iteration’s actual travel time.

Another route choice model developed by Nakayama and Kitamura (1999) assumes that drivers “reason and learn inductively based on cognitive psychology”. The model system is a compilation of “if-then” statements in which the rules governing the statements are systematically updated using algorithms. In essence, the model system represents route choice by a set of rules similar to a production system. The rules for the system represent various cognitive processes. If more than one “if-then” rule applies to a given situation, then the rule that has provided the best directions in the past holds true. The “strength indicator” of each rule is a weighted average of experienced travel times.

Mahmassani and Chang developed a framework to describe the processes governing commuter’s daily departure time decisions in response to experienced travel times and congestion. It was determined that commuter behavior can be viewed as a “boundedly-rational” search for an accepted travel time. Results indicated that a time frame of tolerable schedule delay existed, termed an “indifference band”. This band varied among individuals, and was also effected by the user’s experience with the network.

Jha et. al developed a Bayesian updating model to simulate how travelers update their perceived day-to-day travel time based on information provided by ATIS systems and their previous experience. The framework explicitly modeled the availability and quality of traffic information. They used a disutility function to model driver’s perceived travel time and schedule delay in order to evaluate alternative travel choices. Eventually, the driver chooses an alternative based on utility maximization principle. Finally, the both models are incorporated into a traffic simulator.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

6

2.1 LEARNING MECHANISMS IN ROUTE CHOICE MODELING Generally users cannot foresee the actual travel cost that they will experience during their trip. However, they do anticipate the cost of their travel based on the costs experienced during their previous trips of similar characteristics. Hence, learning and forecasting processes for route choice can be modeled through the use of statistical models applied to path costs experienced on previous trips. There are different kinds of statistical learning models proposed in Cascetta and Canteralla (1993) or Davis and Nihan (1993). There are also other learning and forecasting filters that are empirically calibrated (Mahmassani and Chang, 1985). Most of the previous route choice models in the literature model the learning and forecasting process using one of the two general approaches briefly described below (Cascetta and Canteralla, 1995): • Deterministic or stochastic threshold models based on the difference between the forecasted and actual cost of the alternative chosen the previous day for switching choice probabilities (Mahmassani and Chang, 1985). • Extra utility models for conditional path choice models where the path chosen the previous day is given an extra utility in order to reflect the transition cost to a different alternative (Cascetta and Cantarella, 1993). • Stochastic models that update the probability of choosing a route based on previous experiences according to a specific rule, such as Bayes’ Rule. Stochastic learning is also the learning mechanism adopted in this paper with the exception that the SLA learning rule is a general one and different from Bayes’rule.

3.0 LEARNING AUTOMATA Classical control theory requires a fair amount of knowledge of the system to be controlled. The mathematical model is often assumed to be exact, and the inputs are deterministic functions of time. Modern control theory, on the other hand, explicitly considers the uncertainties present in the system, but stochastic control methods assume that the characteristics of the uncertainties are known. However, all those assumptions concerning uncertainties and/or input functions may not be valid or accurate. It is therefore necessary to obtain further knowledge of the system by observing it during operation, since a priori assumptions may not be sufficient.

It is possible to view the problem of route choice as a problem in learning. Learning is defined as a change in behavior as a result of past experience. A learning system should therefore have the

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

7

ability to improve its behavior with time. “In a purely mathematical context, the goal of a learning system is the optimization of a functional not known explicitly”.

The stochastic automaton attempts a solution of the problem without any a priori information on the optimal action. One action is selected at random, the response from the environment is observed, action probabilities are updated based on that response, and the procedure is repeated. A stochastic automaton acting as described to improve its performance is called a learning automaton (LA). This approach does not require the explicit development of a utility function since the behavior of drivers in our case is implicitly embedded in the parameters of the learning algorithm itself.

The first learning automata models were developed in mathematical psychology. Bush and Mosteller, 1958, and Atkinson et al., 1965, survey early research in this area. Tsetlin (1973) introduced deterministic automata operating in random environments as a model of learning. Fu and colleagues were the first researchers to introduce stochastic automata into the control literature (Fu, 1967).

Recent applications of learning automata to real life problems include control of

absorption columns (Fu, 1967) and bioreactors (Gilbert et al., 1993). Theoretical results on learning algorithms and techniques can be found in recent IEEE transactions (Najaraman et al., 1996, Najim et al., 1994) and in Najim-Poznyak collaboration (Najim et al., 1994).

3.1 LEARNING PARADIGM The automaton can perform a finite number of actions in a random environment. When a specific action α is performed, the environment responds by producing an environment output β, which is stochastically related to the action (Figure 1). This response may be favorable or unfavorable (or may define the degree of “acceptability” for the action). The aim is to design an automaton that can determine the best action guided by past actions and responses. An important point is that knowledge of the nature of the environment is minimal. The environment may be time varying, the automaton may be a part of a hierarchical decision structure but unaware of its role, or the stochastic characteristics of the output of the environment may be caused by the actions of other agents unknown to the automaton.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

Automaton

8

Action α

Action probabilities P

Penalty probabilities C Response

β

Environment

Figure 1. The automaton and the environment

The input action α(n) is applied to the environment at time n. The output β(n) of the environment is an element of the set β=[0,1] in our application. There are several models defined by the output set of the environment. Models in which the output can take only one of two values, 0 or 1, are referred to as P-models. The output value of 1 corresponds to an “unfavorable” (failure, penalty) response, while output of 0 means the action is “favorable.” When the output of the environment is a continuous random variable with possible values in an interval [a, b], the model is named Smodel. The environment where the automaton “lives,” is defined by a triplet {α,c,β} where α is the action set, βrepresents a (binary) output set, and c is a set of penalty probabilities (or probabilities of receiving a penalty from the environment for an action) where each element ci corresponds to one action αi of the action set α. The response of the environment is considered to be a random variable. If the probability of receiving a penalty for a given action is constant, the environment is called a stationary environment; otherwise, it is non-stationary. The need for learning and adaptation in systems is mainly due to the fact that the environment changes with time. Performance improvement can only be a result of a learning scheme that has sufficient flexibility to track the better actions. The aim in these cases is not to evolve to a single action that is optimal, but to choose actions that minimize the expected penalty. For our application, the (automata) environment is non-stationary since the physical environment changes as a result of actions taken.

The main concept behind the learning automaton model is the concept of a probability vector defined (for P-model environment) as

p(n) = {pi (n) ∈{0,1} pi (n) = Pr[α(n) = α i ]}

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

9

where αi is one of the possible actions. We consider a stochastic system in which the action probabilities are updated at every stage n using a reinforcement scheme. The updating of the probability vector with this reinforcement scheme provides the learning behavior of the automata.

3.2 REINFORCEMENT SCHEMES A learning automaton generates a sequence of actions on the basis of its interaction with the environment. If the automaton is “learning” in the process, its performance must be superior to an automaton for which the action probabilities are equal. “The quantitative basis for assessing the learning behavior is quite complex, even in the simplest P-model and stationary random environments (Narendra et al., 1989)”. Based on the average penalty to the automaton, several definitions of behavior, such as expediency, optimality, and absolute expediency, are given in the literature. Reinforcement schemes are categorized based on the behavior type they provide, and the linearity of the reinforcement algorithm. Thus, a reinforcement scheme can be represented as:

p(n + 1) = T[p(n),α(n),β (n)] , where T is a mapping, α is the action, and βis the input from the environment. If p(n+1) is a linear function of p(n), the reinforcement scheme is said to be linear; otherwise it is termed nonlinear. Early studies of reinforcement schemes were centered on linear schemes for reasons of analytical simplicity. In spite of the efforts of many researchers, a general algorithm that ensures optimality has not been found (Kushner et al., 1979). Optimality implies that action αm associated with the minimum penalty probability cm is chosen asymptotically with probability one. Since, it is not possible to achieve optimality in every given situation, a sub optimal behavior is defined, where the asymptotic behavior of the automata is sufficiently close to optimal case.

A few attempts were made to study nonlinear schemes (Chandraeskharan et al.1968, Baba, 1984). Generalization of such schemes to the action case was not straightforward. Later, researchers started looking for the conditions on the updating functions that ensure a desired behavior. This approach led to the concept of absolute expediency. An automaton is said to be absolutely expedient if the expected value of the average penalty at one iteration step is less than the previous step for all steps. Absolutely expedient learning schemes are presently the only class of schemes for which necessary and sufficient conditions of design are available (Chandraeskharan et al.1968, Baba,1984).

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

10

3.3 AUTOMATA AND ENVIRONMENT The learning automaton may also send its action to multiple environments at the same time. In that case, the actions of an automaton result in a vector of feedback values from the environment. Then, the automaton has to “find” an optimal action that “satisfies” all the environments (in other words, all the “teachers”). In a multi-teacher environment, the automaton is connected to N separate teachers.

The action set of the automaton is of course the same for all

teacher/environments. Baba (1984) discussed the problem of a variable-structure automaton operating in many-teacher (stationary and non-stationary) environments. Conditions for absolute expediency are given in his work.

4.0 STOCHASTIC LEARNING AUTOMATA (SLA)

BASED

ROUTE CHOICE

(RC) MODEL - SLA-RC MODEL Some of the major advantages of using stochastic learning automata for modeling user choice behavior can be summarized as follows: 1. Unlike existing route choice models that capture learning process as a deterministic combination of previous days’ and the current day’s experience, SLA-RC model captures the learning process as a stochastic one. 2. The general utility function, V(), used by the existing route choice models is a linear combination of explanatory variables.

However, stochastic learning automata can easily

capture non-linear combinations of these explanatory variables (Narendra and Thathnachar, 1989). This presents an important improvement over existing route choice models since in reality route choice cannot be expected to be a linear combination of the explanatory variables.

4.1 DESCRIPTION OF THE STOCHASTIC LEARNING AUTOMATA ROUTE CHOICE (SLA-RC) MODEL This model consists of two components. These are: • Choice Set: Two types of choice sets can be defined. The first type will be comprised of complete routes between each origin and destination. The user will choose one of these routes every day. This is similar to the pre-trip route choice mechanism. The second type can be comprised of decision points in such a way that the user will choose partial routes to his/her

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

11

destination at each of these decision points. This option is similar to the “en-route” decisionmaking mechanism. • Learning Mechanism:

The learning process will be modeled using stochastic learning

automata that is described in detail in this section.

Let’s assume that there exists an input set X that is comprised of explanatory variables described in Vaughn et al. (1996). Thus X={x1,1, x1,2 … . xt,i} where “t” is the day and “i“ is the individual user or user class. Let’s also assume that there exists an output set D which is comprised of the decisions of route choices, such that D={d1, d2,.......dj}, where j is the number of acceptable routes. This simple system is shown in Figure 2. This system can be made into a feedback system where the effect of user choice on the traffic and vice versa is modeled. This proposed feedback system is shown in figure 3.

O

D

Figure 2. One Origin - One Destination Multiple Route System

Traffic Network Route Choice Probabilities

Updated Travel Times

Figure 3. Feedback mechanism for the SLA-RCM.

For a very simple case with only two routes between an origin and destination and one user class exist, the above system can be seen as a double action system. Then, to update the route choice probability, we can use linear reward-penalty learning scheme proposed in Narendra and Thathnachar (1989). This stochastic automata based learning scheme is selected due to its applicability to the modeling of human learning mechanism in the context of route choice process.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

12

4.1.1 LINEAR REWARD-PENALTY ( L R -P ) SCHEME This learning scheme was first used in mathematical psychology. The idea behind a reinforcement scheme, such as linear reward-penalty ( L R -P ) scheme, is a simple one. If the automaton picks an action α i at instant n and a favorable input (β (n) = 0) results, the action probability pi (n) is increased and all other components of p(n) are decreased. For an unfavorable input β (n) = 1,

pi (n) is increased and all other components of p(n) are decreased. In order to apply this idea to our situation, assume that there are r distinct routes to choose between an origin-destination pair, as seen in Figure 2. Therefore, we can consider this system as a variable-structure automaton with r actions to operate in a stationary environment. A general scheme for updating action probabilities can be represented as follows: If

α (n ) = α i

(i = 1, .....r )

(6)

p j( n + 1) = p j (n ) − g j[ p(n )]

when β ( n) = 0

p j( n + 1) = p j (n ) + h j [p( n)]

when β(n) = 1

for all j ≠ i r

To preserve the probability measure we have

∑ p (n) = 1 j =1

pi (n + 1) = pi (n) +

j

r

∑ g (p(n)) j

so that

when β (n) = 0

(7)

j=1 j≠ i

pi (n + 1) = pi (n) −

r

∑ h (p(n)) j

when β(n) = 1

j=1 j≠ i

The updating scheme is given at every instant separately for that action which is attempted at stage

n in equation (7) and separately for actions that are not attempted in equation (6). Reasons behind this specific updating scheme are explained in Narendara and Thathnachar (1989). In the above equations, the action probability at stage (n + 1) is updated on the basis of its previous value, the action α(n) at the instant n and the input β (n) . In this scheme, p(n+1) is a linear function of p(n), and thus, the reinforcement (learning) scheme is said to be linear.

If we assume a simple

network with one origin destination pair and two routes between this O-D pair, we can consider a learning automaton with two actions in the following form:

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

13 g j (p(n)) = ap j (n)

(8)

and h j (p(n)) = b(1 − p j (n)) In equation (8) a and b are reward and penalty parameters and 0 < a < 1, 0 < b < 1 . If we substitute (8) in equations (6) and (7), the updating (learning) algorithm for this simple two route system can be re-written as follows:

p1 (n + 1) = p1 (n) + a(1 − p1(n)) α(n) = α 1,β(n) = 0 p2 (n + 1) = (1− a)p2 (n) 

(9)

p1 (n + 1) = (1− b)p1 (n)  (n) = 1 α(n) = α1 ,β p2 (n + 1) = p2 (n) + b(1 − p 2 (n))  Equation (9) is in general referred as the general L R -P updating algorithm. From these equations, it follows that if action α i is attempted at stage n, the probability p j (n) j ≠1 is decreased at stage n+1 by an amount proportional to its value at stage n for a favorable response and increased by an

[

]

amount proportional to 1− p j (n) for an unfavorable response.

If we think in terms of route choice decisions, if at day n+1, the travel time on route 1 is less than the travel time on route 2, then we consider this as a favorable response and the algorithm increases the probability of choosing route 1 and decreases the probability of choosing route 2. However, if the travel time on route 1 at day n+1 is higher than the travel time on route 2 the same day, then the algorithm decreases the probability of choosing route 1 and increases the probability of choosing route 2. In this paper, we do not address “departure time choice” decisions. It is assumed that each person departs during the same time interval ∆t. It is also assumed that, for this example, there is one class of user.

5.0. DYNAMIC TRAFFIC / ROUTE CHOICE SIMULATOR The Internet based Route Choice Simulator (IRCS), shown in Figure 4, is the travel simulator developed for this project and was based on one O-D pair and two routes. The simulator is a Java applet designed to acquire data for creating a learning automata model for route choice behavior. The applet can be accessed from any Java-enabled browser over the Internet. Several advantages exist in using web-based travel simulators. First, these simulators are easy to access by different

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

14

types of subjects. Geographic location does not prevent test subjects from participating in the experiments. Second, Internet based simulators permit the possibility of having a large number of subjects partake in the experiment. Internet-based simulators are designed to have a user-friendly GUI, and reduce confusion amongst subjects. Also, Java-based simulators are not limited by hardware, such as the computer type, i.e. Mac, PC, UNIX. Any computer using a Java-enabled web browser can use these simulators. Finally, data processing and manipulation capabilities are significantly increased when using web-based simulators due to the very effective and on-line database tools established for Internet applications.

The site hosting this travel simulator consists of an introductory web page that gives a brief description of the project, and directions on how to properly use the simulator. Following a link on that page brings the viewer to the applet itself. The applet is intended to simulate route choice between two alternative routes. Travel time on route i is the result of a random number picked from a normal distribution with a mean and standard deviation of (µ, σ) and the sum of a nominal average value for travel time on that route as seen in Equation 10.

Travel_ Time(i) = Fixed_ Travel_ Time + Random_Value(µ,σ)

(10)

Users conducting the experiment are asked to make route choice decision on a day-to-day basis. The simulator shows the user the experienced route travel time for each day. Another method for determine travel times is by incorporating the effect of choosing the specific route in terms of extra volume. In this case, travel time is determined as seen in Equation 11.

Travel _ Time(i ) = f(volume)

(11)

The applet can be divided into two sections. The first section is the GUI and the algorithms for determining travel time on each route. Before beginning the experiment, the participant is asked to enter personal and socio-economic data such as name, age, gender, occupation, and income, and user-status. This data will be used in the future to create driver classes and to compare the route choice behavior and learning curves for various classes. Also included in the data survey section are choices for purpose of trip, departure time, desired arrival time, and actual arrival time. For example, if the participant assumed that this experiment simulated a commute from home to work,

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

15

then the participant would realize the importance of arriving at the destination as quickly as possible. Also, the participant could determine how accurate his or her desired arrival time is compared to the actual arrival time.

Figure 4. GUI of Internet Based Route Choice Simulator (IRCS)

The graphics presently incorporated in the applet are GIS maps of New Jersey. These maps can be changed to any image to portray any origin-destination pair. Route choices can be made by clicking on either button on the left panel of the applet. The purpose of having two image panels is two-fold. First, the larger panel on the left can hold a large map of any size, and the map can still remain legible to the viewer. The image panel on the upper right side is intended to zoom in on the particular link chosen by the user on the right panel. For this experiment, such detail is not necessary. However, for future experiments, involving several O-D pairs, this additional capability

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

16

will be beneficial to the applet user. Secondly, the upper right panel contains the participant’s route choice on a particular day, and the corresponding travel time.

The second part of the applet consists of its connection to a database. The applet is connected to an MS Access database using JDBC. The Access database contains a database field for each field in the applet. The database stores all of the information given by the participant, including route choice/travel time combinations on each day. After the participant finishes the experiment, he or she is asked to click the “Save” button in order to permanently save the data to the database. Another important feature of the applet is the ability to query the database from the Internet. In order to query the database, the user simply has to type in the word to be searched in the appropriate text field. Then, the applet generates the SQL query, passes the query to the database, and returns any results. This feature is particularly useful to the designer when searching for particular patterns, values, or participants.

A brief description of the test subjects used in this study is warranted at this point. 66% of the participants are college students, either graduate or undergraduate, while the remaining were professionals in various fields. The participants ranged from the ages of 19 to 26. All came from similar socio-economic backgrounds, and are familiar with the challenges drivers face, specifically route choice. How do we address the issue of similar socio-economic backgrounds? 34% of the participants were female. Finally, all of the participants attempted to determine the shortest route, as can be seen by their comments in the last field, and upon further examination, were all successful. Work is underway to increase the pool of test subjects as well as the capabilities of the travel simulator.

6.0 EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS Several experiments were conducted using the IRCS presented. Normal distribution was used to generate the travel time values for each route. The distribution for the first route had a mean of 45 minutes, and a standard deviation of 10 minutes, whereas the second distribution had a mean of 55 minutes, and a standard deviation of 10 minutes. As can be seen in Figure 4, the given route choices are Route 70 and Route 72 in this experiment, and they are considered to be Route 1 and Route 2 respectively.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

17

The probability of choosing Route 1 at trial i, Pr_Route_1(i), and the probability of choosing Route 2 at trial i, Pr_Route_2(i), are calculated using the following equations:

choicei − 2 + choicei − 1 + choicei + choicei + 1 + choicei + 2 5 choicei 2 + choicei 1 + choicei + choicei 1 + choicei 2 − − + + Pr _ Route_2 ( i) = 5 Pr _ Route_1 (i ) =

(12) (13)

The probabilities of each trial are then plotted for each experiment. Figures 5 and 6 show the plots for Experiment 2 and 5 respectively. As can be seen, all plots show a convergence to the shortest route despite the random travel times for each route. After several trials, the probability of using the route with the shorter average travel time increases.

For each experiment, the respective participant was able to determine the shortest route by at least day 15. Indeed, based on the participant’s comments obtained at the end of the experiment, each participant had correctly chosen the shorter route. If the participant had continued the experiment, the probability of using the shorter route would have converged to one. These results show that the learning process is indeed an iterative stochastic process which can be modeled by iterative updating of the route choice probabilities using a reward-penalty scheme as proposed by our SLARC model presented in section 4.1.1.

Probability of Choosing Route 1 - Experiment #2 1

Probability

0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

5

10

15

20

Day

Figure 5. Route Choice Probability Based on Time (Subject #2)

25

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

18

Probability of Choosing Route 2 - Experiment #5 1

Probability

0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

5

10

15

20

25

Day

Figure 6. Route Choice Probability Based on Time (Subject #5)

6.1 SLA-RC MODEL CALIBRATION The SLA-RC model described in section 4.1.1 by equation (8) has two parameters namely, a and

b , that need to be calibrated. According to the Linear Reward-Penalty ( L R -P ) scheme, when an action is rewarded, the probability of choosing the same route at trial (n + 1) is increased by an amount equal to aP1 (n) and when an action is penalized the probability of choosing same route at trial (n + 1) is decreased by an amount equal to bP1 (n) . Therefore, our task is to determine these

a and b parameters. To achieve this goal, the data set for each experiment is divided into two sub-sets namely, reward and penalty subsets, and a and b parameters are estimated. First, however, a brief explanation of the effects of these parameters on the rate of learning is warranted. The a parameter represents the reward given when the correct action is chosen. In other words, a increases the probability of choosing the same route in the next trial. The effects of this parameter can be seen in Figure 7. As seen in the graph, as a increases, the probability of choosing the correct route also increases. In essence, the rate of learning increases. If a large a value is assigned, then the learning rate will be skewed, and the model will be biased. Parameter b represents the penalty given when the incorrect route is chosen. This parameter will decrease the probability that the incorrect route will be chosen. However, since b is much smaller in relation to a, the effect of a on the learning rate is much greater than that of b.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

19

Probability of Choosing Correct Route

Effect of Parameter "a" on Learning Curve for Ideal Learning 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0

a = 0.02 a = 0.03 a = 0.04 a = 0.05

0

5

10

15

20

25

Day

Figure 7. Effect of Parameter “a” on Learning Curve

As stated previously, the parameters a and b were determined by dividing the data set into two subsets, reward and penalty subsets. Since the travel time for both routes is collected, one can determine the shorter route. If the subject chose the shorter route, then the action is considered favorable, and that trial will be placed in the reward subset. If the subject chose the incorrect route, then that action is considered unfavorable, and that trial will be placed in the penalty subset. Next, the participant’s actual probability distribution is determined. Two subsets are again created for Route 1 and Route 2. For each trial the participant chose Route 1, a binary value of 1 was assigned to the Route 1 subset and a value of 0 was assigned to the Route 2 subset. For each trial the participant chose Route 2, a binary value of 1 will be assigned to the Route 2 subset and a value of 0 will be assigned to the Route 1 subset. Then, the participant’s actual route choice probability distribution will be determined using Equations (11) and (13), and plots such as Figures 5-7 can be obtained. Finally, the pi(n+1) values for the reward and penalty subsets can be calibrated to match the actual probability distribution of the participant.

Based on the Linear Reward-Penalty scheme, the original values of “a” and “b” were 0.02 and 0.002 respectively for learning automata in various disciplines. However, these values provide inaccurate results when applied to route choice behavior. For this model, the parameters had to be derived using the suggested values as a reference. For each individual experiment, parameters “a”

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

20

Table 2. Parameter values for each Experiment Subject Number 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

a values 0.064 0.038 0.042 0.042 0.055 0.053 0.04 0.043 0.043 0.046 0.055 0.02

b values 0.001 0.0018 0.002 0.0015 0.001 0.0015 0.002 0.0015 0.0016 0.002 0.001 0.002

Average Value

0.045

0.0016

and “b” were defined so that pi(n) would closely match the participant’s actual probability distribution. Table 2 shows the results of the parameter analysis for all experiments. As evident in the table, the values of the parameters had a wide range. “a” values ranged from 0.02 to 0.064, while “b” values ranged from 0.001 to 0.002. This wide range is acceptable due to the large variance implicit in route choice behavior and the participant’s learning curve. In other words, participants who learned faster had large “a” values and small “b” values, whereas participants who learned slower had smaller “a” and “b” values. The learning automaton for all experiments is depicted in Figure 8. As can be seen, learning is present in all of the experiments. Also, the learning rate consistently increases, aside from a slight deviation in Experiment #7. The irregular behavior is due to an extreme variance in travel time experienced by this participant around day 15 and can therefore be looked over. Nonetheless, the similarity of the learning curves, plus the steady increase in learning, proves that the derived parameter values accurately reflect learning, and that the large variance in parameter values is acceptable because the learning automaton for each experiment is similar. Based on the parameter values derived in each experiment, an average value was derived for each parameter. The final value for “a” is 0.045, while the final value for “b” is 0.0016.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

21

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

22

6.2 CONVERGENCE PROPERTIES OF ROUTE CHOICE DATA MODELED AS A STOCHASTIC AUTOMATA PROCESS A natural question is whether the updating is performed in a means compatible with intuitive concepts of learning or, in other words, if it is converging to a final solution as a result of the modeled learning process. The following discussion based on Narendra (1974) regarding the expediency and optimality of the learning provides some insights regarding these convergence issues. The basic operation performed by the learning automaton described in equations (6) and (7) is the updating of the choice probabilities on the basis of the responses of the environment. One quantity useful in understanding the behavior of the learning automaton is the average penalty received by the automaton. At a certain stage n, if action α i , i.e. the route i , is selected with probability

pi ( n )

, the average penalty (reward) conditioned on p(n) is given as: (changed the

following equation to E{c from E{β r

M( n) = E{c ( n) p(n )} = ∑ pl ( n)cl

(14)

l=1

If no a priori information is available, and the actions are chosen with equal probability for a random set, the value of the average penalty, M0 , is calculated by:

M0 =

c1 + c 2 + . ... .. ... + cr r

(15)

where, {c1, c2,… .cr} = penalty probabilities

The term of the learning automaton is justified if the average penalty is made less than M0 at least asymptotically. M0 is also called a pure choice automata. This asymptotic behavior, known as expediency, is defined by the Definition 1.

Definition 1 : (Narendra, 1974) A learning automaton is called expedient if

lim E[ M(n)]< M0

n→ ∞

(16)

If the average penalty is minimized by the proper selection of actions, the learning automaton is called optimal, and the optimality condition is given by Definition 2.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

23

Definition 2: (Narendra, 1974) A learning automaton is called optimal if

lim E[ M (n)]= cl

n→ ∞

(17)

where cl = min{c l } l

By analyzing the data obtained on user route choice behavior, one can ascertain the convergence properties of the behavior in terms of the learning automata. Since, the choice converges to the correct one, namely the shortest routes, in the experiments conducted (Figures 5 and 6), it is clear that the

lim E[M(n)] = min{c1 , c2 }

n→ ∞

This satisfies the optimality condition defined in (17). Hence, the learning schemes that are implied by the data for the human subjects, are optimal . Since, they are optimal, they also satisfy the condition that

lim E[M(n)] < M 0

n→ ∞

Hence, the behavior is also expedient .

7.0 SLA-RC MODEL TESTING USING TRAFFIC SIMULATION The stochastic learning automata model developed in the previous section was applied to a transportation network to determine if network equilibrium is reached. The logic of the traffic simulation, written in Java, is shown in the flowchart in Figure 9. The simulation consists of one O-D pair connected by two routes with different traffic characteristics. All travelers are assumed to travel in one direction, from the origin to the destination. Travelers are grouped in “packets” of 5 vehicles with an arrival rate of 3 seconds/packet. The uniform arrival pattern is represented by an arrival array, which is the input for the simulation. The simulation period is 1 hour, assumed to be in the morning peak. Each simulation consists of 94 days of travel.

Each iteration of the simulation represents one day of travel and consists of the following steps, except for Day 1; (a) the traveler arrives at time t, (b) the traveler selects a route based on a random number (RN) generated from a uniform distribution and the comparing it to p1(n) and p2(n), (c) travel time is assigned based upon instantaneous traffic characteristics of the network, (d) p1(n) and p2(n) are updated based upon travel time and route selection, (e) next traveler arrives. This pattern continues for each day until the simulation is ended. As mentioned earlier,

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

24 START

INITIALIZE READ INPUT ARRIVAL ARRAY

DAY = 1 ASSIGN P1 & P2 VALUES TO EACH PACKET DAY = DAY + 1

PACKET = PACKET + 1

YES

NO IF F(RN) TOTAL PACKETS

NO

YES DAY > DAY N?

NO

YES STOP Figure 9. Conceptual Flowchart for Traffic Simulation

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

25

the first day is unique. Travelers arrive according to the arrival array. However, for Day 1, route selection is based upon a random number selected from a uniform distribution. This method is used to ensure that the network is fully loaded before “learning” begins. Route characteristics used in the this simulation are shown below.

“Route 1”

Length

20 km

Capacity

qc = 4000 veh/hr

Travel Time Function t = t0[1+a(q/qc)2] where a = 1.00, t0 = 20 minutes = 0.33 hrs “Route 2”

Length

15 km

Capacity

qc = 2800 veh/hr

Travel Time Function t = t0[1+a(q/qc)2] where a = 1.00, t0 = 15 minutes = 0.25 hrs O-D Traffic

5600 veh/hr

The volume-travel time relationship depicted by the travel time function for both routes can be seen in Figure 10. Route 1 is a longer path and has more capacity. Hence, the travel time curve for this route is flatter, and has a smaller slope. Route 2 is a shorter, and quicker route, with a smaller capacity. Hence, the function is steeper and has a larger slope. Solving the travel time functions for equilibrium conditions yields a flow of 2800 vehicles per hour with a corresponding travel time of 30 minutes on Route 2, and 29.5 minutes on Route 1. The network used here is the same network used in Iida et al. for the analysis of route choice behavior (1992).

The output of the simulation lists each customer’s travel times, route selection, volume on both routes, and route choice probabilities for each day. Output data also includes the total number of vehicles traveling on each route each day. One important point that needs to be emphasized is the fact that our model does not incorporate “departure time” choice of travelers. It is assumed that travelers, packets in this case, depart during the same [t, t + ∆t] time period every morning. This

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

26

assumption is similar to the one made in Nakayama and Kitamura (2000), but it is well recognized as a future enhancement to the model.

Travel Time (minutes)

Figure 10. Travel Time Functions for Route 1 and Route 2 50 45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0

Route 1 Route 2

0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

Volume (veh/hr)

The simulation was conducted ten times. Analysis of the simulation results focused on the learning curves of the packets, and the travel times experienced by the overall network, and each individual packet. First, the network is examined, and then the learning curves of various participants are analyzed. Before the results are presented, the two types of equilibrium by which our results are analyzed must be defined.

Definition 3 (Instantaneous Equilibrium): The network is considered to be in instantaneous equilibrium if the travel times on both routes during any time period ∆t are almost equal. Instantaneous equilibrium exists if

( t , t + ∆t ) ( t , t + ∆t ) tt route _1 (V1 ) - tt route _ 2 (V 2 ) ≤ε

where ( t , t + ∆t ) ( t , t + ∆t ) ttroute _1 (V1 ) & tt route _ 2 (V2 ) are the average travel times during [t, t + ∆t] respectively

ε is a user designated small difference in minutes. This small difference can be considered to be the indifference band, or the variance a traveler allows in the route travel time. Thus, the driver

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

27

will not switch to the other route if the travel time difference between the two routes is within [3,+3] minutes. Definition 4 (Global User Equilibrium): Global user equilibrium is defined as

tt route _ 1 (V1total ) = tt route _ 2 (V2total ) where volume1 and volume2 are the total number of cars that used routes 1 and 2 during the overall simulation period.

The total volumes and corresponding travel times, in minutes, on each route at the end of each simulation are presented in Table 3. As can be seen, the overall travel times for each route are very similar. The largest difference is 1.632 minutes, which can be considered negligible. This fact shows that global user equilibrium, as defined in Definition 4, has been reached.

Table 3. Simulation Results Route 1

Route 2

Global User Volume Travel Time Volume Travel Time Equilibrium 3030 3090 3155 3140 2980 3095 3000 3075 3105

31.161 31.616 32.118 32.001 30.789 31.654 30.938 31.501 31.731

2800 2800 2800 2800 2800 2800 2800 2800 2800

31.877 31.202 30.486 30.650 32.450 31.146 32.219 31.369 31.035

-0.715 0.414 1.632 1.352 -1.660 0.508 -1.282 0.132 0.696

3005

30.975

2800

32.162

-1.187

Next, instantaneous equilibrium, described in Definition 3, is examined. Definition 3 ensures that the difference between route travel times is similar and that each packet is learning the correct route, namely the shortest route for its departure period. A large difference between route travel times shows that the network is unstable because one route’s travel time is much larger than that of the other, and that users are not learning the shortest route for their departure period. Table 4 shows the results of instantaneous equilibrium analysis.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

28

Table 4. Success Rate of Instantaneous Equilibrium Simulation Number

Instantaneous Equilibrium

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

95.000% 86.667% 86.000% 93.333% 79.333% 81.667% 83.083% 90.250% 90.333%

10

95.500%

The data is presented in percent form to reflect the 112,800 choices that are made during the course of the simulation. The percentage of instantaneous equilibrium conditions ranged from 79.333% to 95.500%. This percentage signifies how often instantaneous equilibrium conditions were satisfied throughout each simulation. For simulation 1, 95% of the 112,800 decisions were made during instantaneous equilibrium conditions. These results state that most of the packets choose the shortest route throughout the simulation period.

Figure 11 shows the evolution of instantaneous equilibrium during the course of the simulation. For the first several days, the values are similar, yet incorrect. This can be attributed to the means by which the network was loaded. Each packet required several days to begin the learning process. The slope of each plot represents the learning process. As the packets begin to learn, instantaneous equilibrium is achieved. Finally, each of these simulations achieved and maintained a high-level of instantaneous equilibrium for several days before the simulation ended. This fact ensures network stability and a relationship consistent with Definition 3. If the simulation is conducted for more than 94 days, it is clear that better convergence results in terms of “instantaneous equilibrium” will be obtained.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

29

Evolution of Instanteous Equilibrium 1 0.9

Percentage

0.8 0.7 0.6

Simulation #3 Simulation #8 Simulation #10

0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 0

20

40

60

80

100

Day

Figure 11. Evolution of Instantaneous Equilibrium Finally, the learning curves for several packets individual packets can be seen in Figure 12. All of the curves packets exhibit a high degree of learning. The slope of each curve is steep, which evidences a quick learning rate. The packets in this figure were chosen randomly. The flat portion of three of the curves indicates that learning is not yet taking place; rather, instability exists within the network at that time. However, once stability increases, the learning rate increases greatly. Another proof that learning is taking place is the percent of correct decisions taken by each packet.

For the packets shown in Figure 12, the percentage of correct decisions were mixed. Packet 658 choose correctly 78% of the time it had to make a route choice decision, while packets 1120 (simulation 4) and 320 choose correctly 73% and 75% of the times these packets had to make route choice decisions. Obviously, the correct choice is the shortest route. These learning percentages reflect all 94 days of the simulation. As can be seen from the graph, each packet is consistently learning throughout the entire simulation. The variability in the plots is due to the randomness introduced by the generated random number. On any day of the simulation, the random number generated can cause travelers to choose incorrectly, even though the traveler’s route choice probability (p1 and p2) favors a certain route (i.e. p1 > p2 or p2 > p1).

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

30

Learning Curves for Arbitrary Packets 1 0.9 0.8 Percentage

0.7 Packet 658 Simulation8 Packet 1120 Simulation 4 Packet 1120 Simulation 3 Packet 320 Simulation 9

0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 0

20

40

60

80

100

Day

Figure 12. Learning Curves for Arbitrary Packets

Further proof of the accuracy of the SLA-RC model is gained through comparison of Figures 11 and 12. Figure 11 displays the occurrence of instantaneous network equilibrium, whereas Figure 12 displays the learning curve for various travelers. Based on Figure 11, the occurrence of instantaneous equilibrium is constantly increasing throughout the entire simulation.

As

instantaneous equilibrium increases, so does the learning rate of each packet, as seen in Figure 12.

8.0 LEARNING BASED ON THE EXPECTED

AND

EXPERIENCED TRAVEL

TIMES ON ONE ROUTE The penalty-reward criterion used in the previous section has been modified to test the learning behavior of drivers who do not have full information about the network-wide travel time conditions. This criterion can be described as follows. If, tt exp, i − tt act, i ≤ε route i r

r

Then, the action α l is considered a success.

Otherwise, it is considered a failure and the

probability of choosing route i is reduced according to the linear reward-penalty learning scheme described in the previous sections.

where

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

31

r tt exp, i is the travel time expected by user i on route r.

r r r tt exp, i = tt free -flow + Εi

ε route i = allowed time for early or late arrival r tt act, i = travel time actually experienced by user i on route r

Εir is the random error term representing the personal bias of user i and is obtained from a normal distribution with mean µ = 0, and standard deviation σi, N(µ, σi). This simulation was conducted for six trials. The expected travel time for each traveler was the free-flow travel time for each route (same as before) plus a random error generated from the distribution N(8.5,1.5) minutes. Using these criteria, the following results were obtained.

The range in travel times obtained for each route is shown in Table 5. Travel times on Route 1 ranged from 30.938 minutes to 31.846 minutes, whereas for Route 2, travel times ranged from 30.869 minutes to 32.219 minutes. The difference between overall route travel times ranged from 1.282 minutes to 0.320 minutes, which are relatively small differences in travel time. These results are similar to the results obtained in the previous simulation. It can be stated that “global user equilibrium”, as defined in Definition 4, has been achieved.

Route 1

Route 2

Volume Travel Time Volume Travel Time

Global User Equilibrium

3120 3010 3085 3050 3050 3105 3020 3000

31.846 31.012 31.578 31.312 31.312 31.731 31.086 30.938

2880 2990 2915 2950 2950 2895 2980 3000

30.869 32.105 31.257 31.650 31.650 31.035 31.991 32.219

0.977 -1.093 0.320 -0.338 -0.338 0.696 -0.904 -1.282

3010

31.012

2990

32.105

-1.093

Table 5. Travel Time Differences Table 6 displays the results for instantaneous equilibrium calculations. For this model, instantaneous equilibrium rates were not as consistent, or high, as in the previous model.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

32 Table 6. Success Rate of Instantaneous Equilibrium Simulation Number

Instantaneous Equilibrium

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

85.167% 80.417% 88.750% 95.750% 81.833% 88.750% 91.000% 95.417% 80.917%

10

88.833%

The expected travel time for each customer was random, so that some customers could have smaller expected travel times that were closer to the free-flow travel time, rather than the equilibrium travel time. On other hand, those customers with larger expected travel times were closer to equilibrium travel times. Hence, global user equilibrium was achieved when both types of customers found the shortest route for themselves, since global user equilibrium uses the total volume on each route. However, instantaneous equilibrium was not always achieved because the customers with larger expected travel times could travel on the longer route because the difference between the actual travel time and the expected travel time was still below the minimum. This fact is consistent with the real behavior of drivers. Some drivers choose longer routes because their expected arrival times are quite larger than for other drivers.

Finally, the learning rate of individual packets needs to be addressed. Figure 13 shows the learning rate for four packets. Three of the four packets have a consistent learning rate, and then on day 50, the slope of their learning rate begins to increase dramatically. Packet 340 experienced a high rate of learning in the beginning of the simulation, which then tapered off slightly. However, by the end of the simulation, this packet’s learning rate was relatively equal to the learning rates of the other packets. These four packets choose the correct route approximately 85% of the time they had to make route choice decisions. The random error term for each packet ranged from 7.05 to 8.45 minutes. These results show that each packet learned the shortest route for their expected travel times quickly and efficiently.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

33

Figure 12. Learning Curve for Arbitrary Packets 1 0.9

Percentage

0.8 0.7

Packet 340 Simulation 4 Packet 1100 Simualtion 2 Packet 450 Simulation 3 Packet 765 Simulation 8

0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 0

20

40

60

80

100

Day

Figure 13. Learning Curves for Arbitrary Packets

9.0 CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS In this study, the concept of Stochastic Learning Automata was introduced and applied to the modeling of the day-to-day learning behavior of drivers within the context of route choice behavior. A Linear Reward-Penalty scheme was proposed to represent the day-to-day learning process. In order to calibrate the SLA model, the Internet based Route Choice Simulator (IRCS) was developed. Data collected from these experiments was analyzed to show that the participants’ learning process can be modeled as a stochastic process that conforms to the Linear RewardPenalty Scheme within the Stochastic Automata theory.

The model was calibrated using

experimental data obtained from a set of subjects who participated in the experiments conducted at Rutgers University. Next, a two-route simulation network was developed, and the developed SLARC model was employed to determine the effects of learning on the equilibrium of the network. Two different sets of penalty-reward criteria were used. The results of this simulation indicate that network equilibrium was reached, in terms of overall travel time for the two routes, as defined in Definition 4, and instantaneous equilibrium for each packet, as defined in Definition 3. In the future, we propose to extend this methodology to include departure time choice and multiple origindestination networks with multiple decision points.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

34

REFERENCES 1. Atkinson, R.C., G.H. Bower and E.J. Crothers, An Introduction to Mathematical Learning Theory, Wiley, New York, 1965. 2. Baba, New Topics in Learning Automata Theory and Applications, Lecture Notes in Control and Information Sciences, Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1984. 3. Bush, R.R., and F. Mosteller, Stochastic Models for Learning, Wiley, New York, 1958. 4. Cascetta, E., and Canteralla, G.E., “Dynamic Processes and Equilibrium in Transportation Networks: Towards a Unifying Theory”, Transportation Science, Vol.29, No.4, pp.305-328, 1985. 5. Cascetta, E., and Canteralla, G.E., “A day-to-day and within-day Dynamic Stochastic Assignment Model”, Transportation Research, 25a(5), 277-291 (1991). 6. Cascetta, E., and Canteralla, G.E., “Modeling Dynamics in Transportation Networks”, Journal of Simulation and Practice and Theory 1, 65-91 (1993). 7. Chandrasekharan, B., and D.W.C. Shen, “On Expediency and Convergence in Variable Structure Stochastic Automata,” IEEE Trans. on Syst. Sci. and Cyber., 1968, Vol.5, pp.145149. 8. Davis, N. and Nihan, “Large Population Approximations of a General Stochastic Traffic Assignment Model” Operations Research (1993). 9. Fu, K.S., “Stochastic Automata as Models of Learning Systems,” in Computer and Information Sciences II, J.T. Lou, Editor, Academic, New York, 1967. 10. Gilbert, V., J. Thibault, and K. Najim, “Learning Automata for Control and Optimization of a Continuous Stirred Tank Fermenter,” IFAC Symp. on Adap. Syst. in Ctrl. and Sig. Proc., 1992. 11. Iida, Yasunori, Takamasa Akiyama, and Takashi Uchida, “Experimental Analysis of Dynamic Route Choice Behavior,” Transportation Research Part B, 1992, Vol 26B, No. 1, pp 17-32. 12. Jha, Mithilesh, Samer Madanat, and Srinivas Peeta, “Perception Updating and Day-to-day Travel Choice Dynamics in Traffic Networks with Information Provision,”. 13. Kushner, H.J., M.A.L. Thathachar, and S. Lakshmivarahan, “Two-state Automaton  a Counterexample,” Dec. 1979, IEEE Trans. on Syst., Man and Cyber.s, 1972, Vol. 2, pp.292294.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo

35

14. Mahmassani, Hani S., and Gang-Len Ghang, “Dynamic Aspects of Departure-Time Choice Behavior Commuting System:

Theoretical Framework and Experimental Analysis,”

Transportation Research Record, Vol. 1037. 15. Najim, K., “Modeling and Self-adjusting Control of an Absorption Column,” International Journal of Adaptive Control and Signal Processing, 1991, Vol. 5, pp. 335-345. 16. Najim, K., and A. S. Poznyak, “Multimodal Searching Technique Based on Learning Automata with Continuous Input and Changing Number of Actions,” IEEE Trans. on Syst., Man and Cyber., Part B, 1996, Vol. 26, No. 4, pp.666-673. 17. Najim, K., and A.S. Poznyak, eds., Learning Automata: theory and applications, Elsevier Science Ltd, Oxford, U.K., 1994. 18. Nakayama, Shoichiro and Ryuichi Kitamura, “A Route Choice Model with Inductive Learning,” Submitted for publication in Transportation Research Board. 19. Narendra, K.S., and M.A.L. Thathachar, Learning Automata, Prentice Hall, New Jersey, 1989. 20. Narendra, K.S., “Learning Automata”, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, Cybernetics, pp.323-333, July, 1974. 21. Rajaraman, K., and P .S. Sastry, “Finite Time Analysis of the Pursuit Algorithm for Learning Automata,” IEEE Trans. on Syst., Man and Cyber., Part B, 1996, Vol. 26, No. 4, pp.590598. 22. Sheffi,Y., “Urban Transportation Networks”, Prentice Hall, 1985. 23. Tsetlin, M.L., Automaton Theory and Modeling of Biological Systems, Academic, NY, 1973. 24. Ünsal, C., John S. Bay, and P. Kachroo, “On the Convergence of Linear REpowerd-Penalty Reinforcement Scheme for Stochastic Learning Automata,” submitted to IEEE Trans. in Syst., Man, and Cyber., Part B, August 1996. 25. Ünsal, C., P. Kachroo, and John S. Bay, “Multiple Stochastic Learning Automata for Vehicle Path Control in an Automated Highway System,” submitted to IEEE Trans,. in Syst., Man, and Cyber., Part B, May 1996. 26. Unsal, C., John S. Bay, and Pushkin Kachroo, “Intelligent Control of Vehicles: Preliminary Results on the Application of Learning Automata Techniques to Automated Highway System”, 1995 IEEE International Conference on Tools with Artificial Intelligence.

Ozbay, Datta, Kachroo 27. Unsal, C., Pushkin Kachroo, and John S. Bay , “Simulation Study of Multiple Intelligent Vehicle Control Using Stochastic Learning Automata”, (to be published) Transactions of the International Society of Computer Simulation, 1997. 28. Vaughn, K.M., Kitamura, K., Jovanis, P., “Modeling Route Choice under ATIS in a Multinomial Choice Frameworm”, Transportation research Board, 75th Annual Meeting, Washington D.C., 1996.

36