FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY:

78 downloads 345 Views 56KB Size Report
1994; Ellis, 1984; Joubert & Lecours, 2000; Samuels, LaBerge, & Bremer, 1978; see ..... In J. Everatt (Ed.), Reading and Dyslexia (pp. 1-39). .... Price, C. J., Wise R. J., Watson, J. D., Patterson, K., Howard, D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1994).
A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY: Cognitive Functions and their Neural Basis

Abstract. Reading comprehension is seen as an end product of a number of subprocesses involving the mediation from visual input to phonological representation, in particular, and processes of language comprehension in general which in turn are modulated by aspects of working memory and attention. The neural basis of each of these functionally different subprocesses constituting normal reading is presented. Empirical findings and theories of reading disability are discussed with respect to a possible impairment of one or more of these subprocesses.

1. INTRODUCTION For millions of years, humans have spoken and understood language. Their ability to read and write, however, has been established only in more recent times. From a psycholinguistic perspective, reading must be considered as a secondary process which apart from the visual identification of the word form relies in its consecutive processes on the primary language system (e.g., Perfetti, 1998; Perfetti, Bell & Delaney, 1998; Perfetti & Sandak, 2000). The language system provides the phonological, morphological, semantic and syntactic information over which comprehension processes operate. The processes and components that are specific to reading as compared to auditory language comprehension are (i) the identification of visual features relevant to define letters, (ii) the identification of a visual word form, (iii) and transcoding from orthography to phonology. Thus, there are a number of functionally distinct subprocesses which must be intact to guarantee normal reading. Any research on impaired reading must keep the complexity of the process in mind. The present book discusses the relevant processes and subprocesses involved in reading with respect to their functional relevance and partly with respect to their neural basis allowing the reader to locate past and present empirical findings and theories. In the following, we will first outline the basic functions of normal reading and their neural basis. We will see that the process of normal reading involves the fundamental processes known to constitute the comprehension of spoken sentences plus those basic processes that allow the mediation from visual input to the phonological representation on which higher order linguistic processes are built. Moreover, it will be shown that cognitive processes outside the language domain, namely working memory and attention affect reading processes. After laying these 9 E. Witruk, A. D. Friederici, & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Basic Functions of Language, Reading, and Reading Disability, 921.  2002 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.

10

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

grounds we will try to specify at which of these different processing level and in which domains, normal reading might derail. 2. BASIC FUNCTIONS OF LANGUAGE COMPREHENSION The initial level that becomes active within the language system proper is the phonological processing level. At this level the phonological information is assembled and a phonological representation is built up. This phonological representation allows access to the lexicon and to identify a particular lexical entry that matches the phonological representation (Marslen-Wilson & Warren, 1994; Pisoni & Luce, 1987). At the lexical level, semantic and morphological information, as well as syntactic information (i.e., word class, argument structure), become available. This information is the basis for the relevant processes at the sentence level. At this level the syntactic structure is built up and thematic relations between the different elements in the sentences are assigned to achieve a sentence representation (Frazier, 1987; Just & Carpenter, 1992, see also Swinney & Love, this volume; Saddy, this volume). The further integration of such a sentence representation into the world knowledge may be viewed as the process of understanding in its most general use of the term. Integration of a sentence representation into prior discourse is achieved at the discourse or text level (Kintsch, 1988; Noordman & Vonk, 1999). Each of these processes has been identified to correlate with particular brain areas within a network supporting language comprehension. In brief, these brain areas can be specified as follows. Phonological processes have been located at an integrated network consisting of the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus/sulcus and the inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann Area (BA) 44), (Démonet et al., 1992; Zatorre, Evans, Meyer, & Gjedde, 1992; Price et al, 1992). Lexical-semantic processes have been reported to correlate with activation in the middle temporal gyrus and the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45/47), (Fiez, 1997; Kapur et al., 1994; ThompsonSchill, D’Esposito, Aquirre, & Farah, 1997). Syntactic processes were shown to involve the anterior part of the superior temporal gyrus and the inferior frontal gyrus (inferior part of BA 44 and the adjacent frontal operculum), (Caplan, Alpert, & Waters, 1998; Friederici, Meyer, & von Cramon, 2000; Just, Carpenter, Keller, Eddy, & Thulborn, 1996; Stromswold, Caplan, Alpert, & Rauch, 1996). Discourse level processes, furthermore, involve brain areas in the frontal median wall (Ferstl & von Cramon, 2001). Although all these areas show their activation predominantly in the left hemisphere, right homologue areas are reported to be simultaneously active, in particular, at the sentential level when the input is presented auditorily. Recent studies have shown that the increased right hemisphere activation is partly due to prosodic information present in spoken sentences (Meyer, Alter, & Friederici, in press). Furthermore, a comparison between auditory and visual studies at the sentence level reveals that the left inferior frontal gyrus is always active when the input is visual, but also, albeit less consistently when it is auditory (Michael, Keller, Carpenter, & Just, 2001). This seems to suggest that during sentence reading phonological processes (located in the superior part of BA 44) become active.

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

11

3. BASIC FUNCTIONS OF READING In most general terms reading can be characterized as language comprehension, including all aspects introduced above, plus visual decoding (Everatt et al., 1999). However, reading cannot be defined as simply the sum, but rather as an interaction of these components (e.g., Perfetti & Sandak, 2000). Furthermore, the visual processes involved in reading are not identical to those of visual object recognition. Instead, they are highly specialized to a fast and accurate desymbolization of visual icons (Deacon, 2000). In this respect, reading can be characterized as a highly automated desymbolization process. Different levels of the visual domain are involved in this desymbolization process. At the visual feature level, different features relevant for the identification of a letter such as lines, angles and contours have to be processed to allow the activation of letters (Massaro, 1998). At the visual word from level, the identification of visual word forms takes place (Seymour & Evans, 1993). Dual Route Models of word reading assume a direct route from the visual word form to the word’s meaning for high frequency words and a second route for low frequency words (Coltheart & Rastle, 1994; Ellis, 1984; Joubert & Lecours, 2000; Samuels, LaBerge, & Bremer, 1978; see also Jimenez-Gonzalez, this volume). This latter route to the lexicon proceeds via a grapheme-to-phoneme conversion rule system in which individual letters are mapped onto phonological units before these are assembled into a phonological word form. All models agree that the latter system certainly has to be activated during pseudoword reading. However, the different models are less univocal as to whether low frequency words are the only real words processed by means of grapheme-to-phoneme conversion rules (Coltheart & Rastle, 1994) or whether the identification of a word always involves the immediate co-activation of graphemic and phonological constituents (Booth, Perfetti & MacWhinney, 1999). The brain areas which were identified to be involved in word reading are the following. Brain areas in the occipital lobe specified to detect line orientation and contours are involved in the processing of the visual features of letters. Within the occipito-temporal brain areas the fusiform gyrus is activated when reading between words and pronounceable non-words is compared (Fiebach, Schlesewsky, & Friederici, in press; Herbster, Mintun, Nebes, & Becker, 1997). The middle part of the fusiform gyrus has been taken to be the locus of the visual word form area, in which a perceptually invariant higher-order orthographic unit (i.e., a visual word form) is computed from the visual input (Cohen et al., 2000). Note that this brain area in the fusiform gyrus, although relevant for reading, is not reading-specific as it is also activated during visual face recognition (Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2000), and more generally when experts process highly familiar objects of the category of their expertise (Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore, & Anderson, 2000). Fiebach et al. (in press) proposed that this notion of a category-specific recognition component could be extended to written words - a category for which any normal literate adult is an expert. The left inferior frontal gyrus and the anterior insula have been associated with phonological processing, phonological retrieval and phonemic analysis (for review see Fiez & Petersen, 1998). Activation of this area was not only observed during

12

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

pronunciation tasks, but also during lexical decision (Fiebach et al., in press). Increased activation of the superior part of the left pars opercularis (BA 44) was observed for low frequency words and for pseudo-words compared to real words suggesting that lexical access was mediated by phonological information for the former word types. Thus, it appears that this area can be viewed to subserve grapheme-to-phoneme conversion. Note, however, that this area is involved in phonological processes both during production and perception. The left middle temporal gyrus known to be involved in auditory language processing has been observed as part of the neural network of word reading. The combined data from different studies suggest that activation in this area can be associated with the activation of phonological word forms (Hagoort, Brown, & Osterhout, 1999; Price et al., 1994) which are part of the word’s lexical entry. Price (2001) recently pointed out that the combined results from a number of brain imaging studies indicate that there are no brain areas that are specific to reading. Rather it appears that learning to read involves “establishing associate connections between object processing areas in the visual cortex and speech processing areas in temporal and frontal cortices” (Price, 2001). This result is compatible with the notion that from an evolutionary point of view, reading is a secondary system which is not associated with areas specific to reading, but which rather recruits brain areas primarily subserving other functions. 4. ASPECTS OF MEMORY AND ATTENTION DURING READING Besides these fundamental processes of reading and language comprehension, cognitive processes outside these domains appear to be necessary to guarantee normal reading, namely working memory capacity and attention. Working memory has been identified as a main factor determining reading comprehension (Just & Carpenter, 1992; MacDonald, Just, & Carpenter, 1992; but see Waters & Caplan, 1996). This view is based on a large number of behavioral studies showing that sentence comprehension performance decreases when the individual working memory capacity is low, as assessed by the Reading Span Test or when concurrent working memory capacity is high (e.g., Carpenter, Miyake, & Just, 1994). Event-related brain potential studies support the view that syntactic comprehension is constrained by individual working memory constraints (Friederici, Steinhauer, Mecklinger, & Meyer, 1998) and by concurrent working memory load (Vos, Gunter, Schriefers, & Friederici, 2001). The underlying factor for the performance difference in high and low span readers, however, is defined differently in these studies. While Carpenter and Just (1992) claim that low span readers’ difficulty to process syntactically ambiguous sentences is due to an inability to keep the two possible underlying syntactic structures active, others hold that their problem lies in the inability to commit themselves early to the preferred structure and in order to keep the working memory load low (Friederici et al., 1998). Whatever the valid interpretation of the underlying processes is, the observation is that the correlation between the individual working memory capacity and of reading comprehension ability is high. A correlation between working memory and

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

13

reading has also been demonstrated for the level of lexical processing. It was found that the resolution of lexical ambiguity is influenced by working memory (Gernsbacher & Faust, 1991; Miyake, Just, & Carpenter, 1994). As mentioned before, two different exp lanations have been put forward. The first states that low span readers are not able to keep both reading of and ambiguous word active (Miyake et al., 1994), while the other claims that low span readers are unable to actively suppress the irrelevant reading. This correlation between lexical aspects of comprehension and working memory has not been evaluated using electrophysiological measures so far. However, working memory for single words has been investigated in recent brain imaging studies and so has the relation between working memory and syntactic processing. Aspects of lexical-semantic working memory were shown to be correlated with activation in left temporal regions and in the left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45/47), (Paulesu, Frith, & Frackowiak, 1993; Gabrieli, Brewer, & Poldrack, 1998; Wiggs, Weisberg, & Martin, 1999). Aspects of syntactic working memory also activate parts of the left inferior frontal gyrus, namely BA 44/45 (Fiebach et al., in press). While this region was shown to be active when syntactic complex sentences requiring working memory resources are processed in English (Just et al., 1996; Stromswold et al., 1996). Fiebach et al. (in press) using German were able to show that this brain area selectively reacts to working memory constraints rather than to syntactic complexity per se. Note, however, that these two factors are compounded in natural sentences. Thus, the combined data indicate that working memory as tested by the Reading Span Test (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980) influences comprehension performance during reading (see also Osaka & Osaka, this volume). Moreover, they indicate that adjacent, but separable brain areas in the inferior frontal and temporal cortex support lexical and syntactic aspects of working memory. What seems noteworthy is that working memory capacity interacts with so-called higher levels of processing during reading, namely lexical and syntactic processing, and not with lower level processes such as visual perception and phoneme-tographeme conversion. Attention on the other hand appears to be more likely to interact with lower as well as higher level reading processes (McCarthy & Nobre, 1993; Otten, Rugg, & Doyle, 1993; Smid, Jakob, & Heinze, 1999). Taken together, reading is a complex process that involves lower level perceptual processes and central language processes which interact with aspects of attention and working memory. These processes were discussed to differ not only functionally, but also at the level of neuronal systems. It is not unlikely that reading disability may have its origin in each of these different systems. Therefore, it is not surprising that a number of different theories and models of reading disability have been proposed.

14

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN 5. READING DISABILITY RESEARCH

Current theories and models of reading disability are anything but univocal. The understanding of reading disability depends on the understanding of the reading process as such (Rayner, 1993), the definition of dyslexia (Toth & Siegel, 1994), and methodological approaches (see e.g., Miles & Miles, 1999). As discussed, reading is a complex cognitive technique which requires the coordination of a series of subfunctions which can be characterized as visual functions, such as configurational (feature) and orthographic (word form) analyses, and verbal (language) functions, such as phonological, semantic and syntactic coding and decoding. From this point of view, models of reading disability can be subdivided grossly into those assuming visual deficits and those assuming language-based (linguis tic including phonological) deficits as causal factors for reading disability (cf. Vellutino, 1977; Vellutino & Scanlon, 1998). Early models of word-blindness (Kussmaul, 1878) and strephosymbolia (Orton, 1925), which were dominant until the late 70’s, are sometimes characterized to be (somehow old-fashioned) visual deficit models (Vellutino, 1977; Vellutino & Scanlon, 1987, 1998). However, these models, as well as recent adaptations (Corballis & Beale, 1993; see also Brendler & Lachmann, 2001; Lachmann, this volume), already suppose a failure in the binding of visual and phonological/semantic representations to cause reading problems (reversals). Thus, the reason for a failure in reading is assumed to be a failure in the coordination of the subfunctions involved. This deficit is supposed to lead to a faulty integration of visual and auditory information. The influential studies of Liberman, Shankweiler, Orlando, Harris, & Bell Berti (1971), Vellutino (1977, 1979) and Fisher, Liberman and Shankweiler (1978) initiated the “phonological turnaround” (see Lachmann, this volume) in the field of reading disability research. As a result, to this day most cognitive explanations of reading disability are based on the assumption of phonological deficits within the language processing system (Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Snowling, 2001; Vellutino & Scanlon, 1987; see the chapters of Uhry, Gregg et al., Jimenez-Gonzalez, Fawcett, DahlgrenSandberg and Kujala, this volume). Reading is understood as a primary linguistic skill (Liberman, 1983). Visual models of reading disability are, for instance, those which assume a deficit - or better a difference - (cf. Stein, this volume) in the temporal integration of visual information, (Stanley & Hall, 1973) or more specifically, in the coordination between the transient and the sustained pathway (Breitmeyer & Ganz, 1976; Livingstone & Hubel, 1987) in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the visual system (Lovegrove, Martin & Slaghuis, 1986; Lovegrove, 1993; see the chapters of Stein, Galaburda, and Talcott & Witton, this volume). A failure of such a coordination averts an adequate and fast setup of visual representations. However, there are also models which assume a general temporal integration deficit, which is not only effective to the vis ual system, but also within the auditory modality (e.g., Farmer & Klein, 1995; Tallal, 1984). On the other hand, also those models have to be counted as purely visual, which assume reading disability as a result of deficits in visual memory, that is, not in building up, but in operating with visual representations (Willows, Kruk, & Corcos, 1993 for review; Witruk, Ho, & Schuster, this volume).

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

15

These differences between the models point to another important distinction that can be made between the models of reading disability. There are models assuming that problems occur in early processing (low level deficits), and those assuming that problems occur in late processing (higher level deficits including memory) of either visual or verbal information, respectively (e.g., visual memory: Willows et al., 1993, for a review; visual perceptual: Breitmeyer, 1993, for a review; e.g., phonetic memory: e.g., Katz, Healy, & Shankweiler, 1983; temporal integration of phonetic information: Tallal, 1984). For instance, Tallal (1984) argues that deficits in phonological representations may be due to a low-level deficit in rapid processing of auditory information. Others argue resolutely against such an explanation (Snowling, 2001). Hulme (1988) reviews evidence against the assumption of low-level deficits within the visual domain measured by visible persistence, contrast sensitivity and flicker rate sensitivity in disabled readers, and the interpretation as a difference in functioning of the transient system (see the chapters of Stein, Galaburda, and Talcott & Witton, this volume). In some cases, the discussion about late versus early deficits can be characterized as the question whether late processing deficits, and finally reading by itself, is the effect of, or the cause for, early processing deficits (Hulme, 1988; Snowling, 2001). Alone, the fact of correlation between measures of early or late processing and reading performance is not sufficient to decide this question. Furthermore, models have to be mentioned which emphasize the importance of more general or guiding cognitive abilities for functioning reading, such as visual attention and eye movement control (see Radach, Inhoff, & Heller and Starr, Kampe, Miller, & Rayner, this volume), stereo vision (Stein, Richardson, & Fowler, 2000) or other - perhaps less reading specific cognitive abilities (see Witruk, Ho, & Schuster, this volume). However, even though these models appear to be visual approaches at first glance, differences found between normal and poor/disabled readers, e.g., in the pattern of saccades, the number of regressions and the location of the optimal viewing point (see Everatt et al., 1999 for review; see the chapters of Starr et al. and Radach et al., this volume) may not necessarily require an interpretation in favor of the visual hypothesis of reading disability, but can also be explained in terms of differences in language processing (e.g., Kennedy, 1987; Everatt & Underwood, 1994). A most important distinction of models to be made is that of monocausal versus multicausal explanations of reading disability. Orton’s (1925) traditional concept (strephosymbolia), for instance, is a monocausal theory, that is, reversal errors are defined as the cardinal symptom of an abnormal cerebral dominance and a failure in suppressing mirror image representations during the integration of visual and phonological information. The aforementioned studies of Liberman et al., (1971) and Fisher et al. (1978) do not doubt the significance of reversal errors in reading disability. Rather, they dispute the role of reversals as the cardinal symptom and the monocausality of the theory of strephosymbolia. More recent explanations of reversal errors, such as that of the concept of symmetry generalization (Brendler & Lachmann, 2001; Lachmann, this volume; see also Lachmann & van Leeuwen, in press), are an integrated part of a multicausal model.

16

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

The discussion about multi - vs. monocausality is strongly related to the discussion about the definition of reading disability (or dyslexia). The so-called discrepancy definition of dyslexia is based on a significant discrepancy between the observed reading achievement and the reading achievement expected by the students general intelligence (see e.g., Jiménez-Gonzales, this volume). This symptom-based definition (symptom principle, cf. Tonnessen, 1995) is especially criticized by those who define reading disability as caused (causality principle, cf. Tonnessen, 1995) by a purely phonological deficit (Miles, 1991; Siegel, 1998; Snowling, 1998, 2001; Toth & Siegel, 1994; Wimmer, Landerl, & Frith, 1999). In contrast, most models which concede visual differences in disabled readers do not doubt possible phonological deficits in all, or a subgroup of disabled readers (Becker, Lachmann, & Elliott, 2001; Doering, Trites, Patel, & Fiedorowicz, 1981; Satz & Morris 1981; Slaghuis, Twell, & Kingston, 1996;). Most influential in this respect was Boder’s (1968, cited in Boder, 1971) distinction between dyseidetics (10% of the sample) and dysphonetics (67% of the sample). Dyseidetics were characterized to have problems in processing visual word gestalts and to stick to phonological strategies even for highly frequent words. Dysphonetics were observed to fail in adequate use of phonological patterns of words and to prefer visual strategies even for unfamiliar words. However, almost a quarter of the sample (23%) showed a mixed pattern of deficits and could not be classified within this dichotomy. A body of evidence for dyslexia subtypes has been collected since then whose significance, however, is called into question by others. Snowling (2001), for instance, argues that differences in non-word reading are simply due to the severity of the phonological deficit, and that parameters of visual processing (perceptual and memory) may predict reading ability only rarely and if then because of its role as sources of compensation of the causal phonological deficit (see also Stanovich, 1994; Stanovich, Siegel, Gottardo, Chiappe, & Sidhu, 1997). Others argue that a failure in learning to read may be caused by problems in any of the subfunctions (including attention and guiding cognitive abilities) and their interaction and thus, that reading disability or dyslexia labels a variety of problems (syndrome) in reading (Rayner, 1993; Lachmann, this volume). In summary, the field of reading disability research seems to be quite heterogeneous. There is some contradicting experimental evidence, and certainly contradicting interpretations of the same experimental results and, most importantly, there is no consistency about the definition of reading disability or about whether distinct groups of normal and reading disabled children even exist (Bryant & Impey, 1986; Stanovich, 1994; see also Jimenez-Gonzales, this volume). However, this is not surprising given that the process of reading includes many subprocesses each separable functionally and neurophysiologically. Different approaches have been used to understand the phenomenon of reading disability abetting the heterogeneity of the explanations and theories. Educational scientists and school teachers have concentrated on individual as well as general patterns of reading problems in order to develop special teaching programs (in the tradition of Gates, 1936). Neurologists and cognitive neuroscientists focused on neurophysiological differences between normal and disabled readers (see e.g., the chapters of Kujala and Fawcett, this volume). Cognitive psychologists using

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

17

experimental procedures tried to test the separate functions which are assumed to cause the failure in reading. However, for an ultimate understanding of reading disability, all these different approaches are important. The bridge between the different research domains must be built to foster an integrative theory of reading dis ability. The present book, which brings together researchers from the neurosciences, from language acquisition, from adult language processing and reading with those investigating reading disabilities lays a first foundation for such a bridge. 6. AFFILIATIONS Dr. Angela D. Friederici Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience PO. Box 500 355 D-04303 Leipzig, Germany e-mail: [email protected] Dr. Thomas Lachmann University of Leipzig Seeburgstrasse 14-20 D-04103 Leipzig, Germany e-mail: lachmann@u ni-leipzig.de.

7. REFERENCES Becker, C., Lachmann, T., & Elliott, M. (2001). Evidence for impaired integration-segmentation processes and effects of ocillatory synchrony on stimulus coding in dyslexics. In E. Sommerfeld, R. Kompass, & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Fechner Day 2001. Proceedings of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 273-278). Lengerich, Berlin: Pabst. Boder, E. (1971). Developmental dyslexia: Prevailing diagnostic concepts and a new diagnostic approach. In H. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (pp. 293-321). New York: Grune & Stratton. Booth, J. R., Perfetti, C. A., & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Quick, automatic, and general activation of orthographic and phonological representations in young readers. Developmental Psychology, 35, 3-19. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-521. Breitmeyer, B. G. (1993). The roles of sustained (P) and transient (M) channels in reading and reading disability. In S. F. Wright, & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation. Studies in visual information processing (pp. 13-31). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier. Breitmeyer, B. G., & Ganz, L. (1976). Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. Psychological Review, 83, 1-36. Brendler, K., & Lachmann, T. (2001). Letter reversals in the context of the Functional Coordination Deficit Model. In E. Sommerfeld, R. Kompass, & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Fechner Day 2001. Proceedings of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 308-313). Lengerich, Berlin: Pabst. Bryant, P. E., & Impey, L. (1986). The similarities between normal readers and developmental and acquired dyslexics. Cognition, 24, 121-137. Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1998). Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 541-552.

18

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

Carpenter, P. A., Miyake, A., & Just, M. A. (1994). Working memory contraints in comprehension: Evidence from individual differences, aphasia, and aging. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 1075-1122). San Diego: Academic Press. Cohen, D., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehericy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Henaff, M.-A., & Michel, F. (2000). The visual word form area. Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split -brain patients. Brain, 123, 291307. Coltheart, M., & Rast le, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 1197-1211. Corballis, M. C., & Beale, I. L. (1993). Orton revisited: Dyslexia, laterality, and left-right confudion. In D. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 57-73). Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450-466. Deacon, T. W. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on language and brain plasticity. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33, 273-291. Démonet, J. F., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespoulous, J. L., Wise, R., Rascol, A., & Frackowiak, R. (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain, 115, 1753-1768. Doehring, D. G., Trites, R. L., Patel, P. G., & Fiedorowicz, C. A. M. (1981). Reading disabilities: The interaction of reading, language and neuropsychological deficits. New York: Academic Press. Ellis, A. W. (1984). Reading, writing and dyslexia: A cognitive analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Everatt, J., McCorquidale, B., Smith, J., Culverwell, F. Wilks, A. Evans, D., Kay, M, & Baker, D. (1999). Association between reading ability and visual processes. In J. Everatt (Ed.), Reading and Dyslexia (pp. 1-39). London: Routledge. Everatt, J., & Underwood, G. (1994). Individual differences in reading subprocesses. Language and Speech, 37, 283-297. Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychnomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460-493. Ferstl, E. C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The locus of coherence and cohesion in text comprehension: An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 325-340. Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (in press). An ERP investigation of syntactic working memory during the processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language. Fiez, J. A. (1997). Phonology, semantics, and the role of the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 79-83. Fiez J. A., & Petersen S. E. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 95, 914-921. Fisher, F. W., Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1978). Reading Reversals and Developmental Dyslexia: A Further Study. Cortex, 14, 496-510. Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII (pp. 559-586). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: An event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language, 74, 289-300. Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K., Mecklinger, A., & Meyer, M. (1998). Working memory constraints on syntactic ambiguity resolution as revealed by electrical brain responses. Biological Psychology, 47, 193-221.

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

19

Gabrieli, J. D. E., Brewer, J. B., & Poldrack, R. A. (1998). Images of medial temporal lobe functions in human learning and memory. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 70, 275283. Gates, A. I. (1936). The improvement of reading: A program of dia gnostic and remedial methods. New York: Macmillan. Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191-197. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. (1991). The role of suppression in sentence comprehension. In G. B. Simpson (Ed), Understanding word and sentence. Advances in psychology, No. 77. (pp. 97-128). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Osterhout, L. (1999). The neurocognition of syntactic processing. In C. Brown, & P. Hagoort (Eds.), Neurocognition of Language (pp. 273-316). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences, 4, 223-232. Herbster, A. N., Mintun, M. A., Nebes, R. D. Becker, J. T. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow during word and nonword reading. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 84-92. Hulme, C. (1988). The Implausibility of Low-Level Visual Deficits as a Cause of Children’s Reading Difficulties. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 3, 369-374. Joubert, S. A., & Lecours, A. R. (2000). The role of sublexical graphemic processing in reading. Brain & Language, 72, 1-13. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149. Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114-116. Kapur, S., Craik, F. I. M., Tulving, E., Wilson, A. A., Houle, S., & Brown, G. (1994). Neuroanatomical correlates of encoding in episodic memory: Levels of processing effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 91, 2008-2011. Katz, R. B., Healy, A. F., & Shankweiler, D. (1983). Phonetic coding and order memory in relation to reading proficiency: A comparison of short -tem memory for temporal and spatial order information. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 229-250. Kennedy, A. (1987). Eye movements, reading skill and spatial code. In J. Beech, & A. Colley (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to reading (pp. 102-142). Chichester: Wiley. Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163-182. Kussmaul, A. (1878). Word-deafness and word-blindness. In H. v. Ziemssen (Ed.), Cyclopaedia of the practice of medicine. London: Sampson Row, Maston, Searle & Rivingston. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (in press). Memory-guided Inference in Same-Different Comparison Tasks. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schroeger, & H. J. Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation. Hilldale, NJ: Erlbaum. Liberman, I. Y. (1983). A language -orientated view of reading and its disabilities. In H. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (pp 81-101). New York: Grune & Stratton. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Orlando, C., Harris, K. S., & Bell Berti, F. (1971). Letter Confusions and Reversals of Sequence in the Beginning Reader: Implications for Orton’s Theory of Developmental Dyslexia. Cortex, 7, 127-142. Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 3416-3468. Lovegrove, W. J. (1993). Do dyslexics have a visual deficit? In S. F. Wright, & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation. Studies in visual information processing (pp. 33-49). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier. Lovegrove, W., Martin, F., & Slaghuis, W. (1986). A Theoretical and Experimental Case for a Visual Deficit in Specific Reading Disability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 2, 225-267.

20

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 56-98. Marslen-Wilson, W., & Warren, P. (1994). Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review, 101, 653-675. Massaro, D. W. (1998). Models for reading letters and words. In D. Scarborough, & S. Sternberg (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive psychology. Methods, models and conceptual issues (pp. 301-364). Cambridge: MIT Press. McCarthy, G., & Nobre, A. C. (1993). Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials, 88, 210219. Meyer, M., Alter, K, & Friederici, A. D. (in press). Towards the cerebral substrates of sentencelevel syntactic and prosodic processing. Speech and Communication. Michael, E. B., Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). fMRI investigation of sentence comprehension by eye and by ear: Modality fingerprints on cognitive processes. Human Brain Mapping, 13, 239-252. Miles, E. (1991). Visual dyslexia/ auditory dyslexia: Is this a valuable distinction to make? In M. Snowling, & M. Thompson (Eds.), Dyslexia: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 195-203). London: Whurr. Miles, T. R., & Miles, E. (1999). Dyslexia: A hundred years on. Buckingham: Open University Press. Miyake, A., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1994). Working memory constraints on the resolution of lexical ambiguity: Maintaining multiple interpretations in neutral contexts. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 175-202. Noordman, L. G. M., & Vonk, W. (1999). Discourse comprehension. In A. D. Friederici (Ed.), Language comprehension: A biological perspective, 2 nd Edition (pp. 230-263). Berlin/ Heidelberg: Springer Verlag. Orton, S. T. (1925). “Word-Blindness” in School Children. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 14, 5, 581-615. Otten, L. J., Rugg, M. D., & Doyle, M. C. (1993). Modulation of event-related potentials by word repetition: The role of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology, 30, 559-571. Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1993). The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature, 362, 342-345. Perfetti, C. A. (1998). Comprehending written language: a blueprint of the reader. In C. M. Brown, & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 167-208). Oxford: University Press. Perfetti, C. A., Bell, L. C., & Delaney, S. M. (1998). Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory & Language, 27, 59-70. Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education, 5, 32-50. Pisoni, D. B., & Luce, P. A. (1987). Acoustic - phonetic representations in word recognition. Cognition, 25, 21-52. Price, A. (2001). Anatomical and Cognitive Models of Reading: From Dejerine to Functional Imaging. Paper presented at the Neural Basis of Language Conference, Groningen, July 9 11, 2001. Price, C. J., Wise, R. J., Ransay, S., Friston, K., Howard, D., Patterson, K., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1992). Regional response within the human auditory cortex when listening to words. Neuroscience Letters, 146, 179-182. Price, C. J., Wise R. J., Watson, J. D., Patterson, K., Howard, D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1994). Brain activity during reading: The effects of exposure duration and task. Brain, 117, 12551269.

FROM LANGUAGE TO READING AND READING DISABILITY

21

Rayner, K. (1993). Visual porocessing in reading: Directions for research and theory. In D. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 475-480). Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum. Samuels, S. J., LaBerge, D., & Bremer, C. D. (1978). Units of word recognition: Evidence for developmental changes. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 17, 715-720. Satz, P., & Morris, R. (1981). Learning disability subtypes: A review. In F. J. Pirozzolo, & M. C. Wittrock (Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading (pp. 109-141). New York: Academic Press. Seymour, P. H. K., & Evans, H. M. (1993). The visual (orthographic) processor and developmental dyslexia. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 347-376). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Siegel, L. S. (1998). Phonological processing deficits and reading disabilities. In J. L. Metsala, & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 141-160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Slaghuis, W. L., Twell, A. J., & Kingston, K. R. (1996). Visual and language processing disorders are concurrent in dyslexia and continue into adulthood. Cortex, 32, 413-438. Smid, H. G. O. M., Jakob, A., & Heinze H. J. (1999). An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. Psychophysiology, 36, 264-279. Snowling, M. (1998). Dyslexia as a phonological deficit: Evidence and implications. Child Psychology & Psychiatry Review, 3, 4-11. Snowling, M. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 37-46. Stanley, G., & Hall, R. (1973). Short -term information processing in dyslexics. Child Development, 44, 841-844. Stanovich K. E. (1994). Annotation: Does Dyslexia Exist?. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat.,35, 4, 579-595. Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., Gottardo, A., Chiappe, P., & Sidhu, R. (1997). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Differences in phonological and orthographic coding. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 115-141). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stein, J. F., Richardson, A. J., & Fowler, M. S. (2000). Monocular occlusion can improve binocular control and reading in dyslexics. Brain, 123, 164-170. Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52, 452-473. Tallal, P. (1984). Temporal or phonetic processing deficit in dyslexia? That is the question. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 167-169. Thompson-Schill, S. L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 94, 14792-14797. Tonnessen, F. E. (1995). On defining “dyslexia.” Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 39, 139-156. Toth, G., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). A critical evaluation of the IQ-achievement discrepancy based definition of dyslexia. In K. P. van den Bos, & L. S. Siegel (Eds), Current directions in dyslexia research (pp. 45-70). Lisse, NL: Swets & Zeitlinger. Vellutino, F. R. (1977). Alternative conceptualizations of dyslexia: Evidence in support of a verbal-deficit hypotheses. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 334-354. Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321-363 Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1998). Research in the study of reading disability: What have we learned in the past four decades? Manuscript of an invited paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego.

22

A. D. FRIEDERICI & T. LACHMANN

Vos, S. H., Gunter, T. C., Schriefers, H., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic parsing and working memory: The effects of syntactic complexity, reading span, and concurrent load. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 65-103. Waters G. S., & Caplan D. (1996). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 5179. Wiggs, C. L., Weisberg, J., & Martin, A. (1999). Neural correlates of semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 37, 103-118. Willows, D. M., Kruk, R. S., & Corcos, E. (1993). Are there differences between disabled and normal readers in their processing of visual information?. In D. M Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 265-287). Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum. Wimmer, H., Landerl, K., & Frith, U. (1999). Learning to read German: Normal and impaired acquisition. In M. Harris, & G. Hatano (Eds), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge studies in cognitive and perceptual development (pp. 34-50). New York: Cambridge University Press. Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., & Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846-849.