Increased sensitivity for angry faces in depressive ... - Semantic Scholar

1 downloads 0 Views 244KB Size Report
Jan 27, 2010 - trations of the antidepressant agents reboxetine and citalopram have, for instance, been shown to reduce biases for negative information and ...
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology (2010), 13, 1155–1161. Copyright f CINP 2010 doi:10.1017/S1461145710000660

ARTICLE

Increased sensitivity for angry faces in depressive disorder following 2 weeks of 2-Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the right parietal cortex Dennis J. L. G. Schutter1, Jack van Honk1,2, Martin Laman3, Anton C. Vergouwen4 and Frank Koerselman4,5 1

Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, J-Block Groote Schuur Hospital Observatory, South Africa 3 Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, St Lucas Andreas Hospital, Amsterdam, The Netherlands 4 Department of Psychiatry, St Lucas Andreas Hospital, Amsterdam, The Netherlands 5 Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Centre Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands 2

Abstract According to the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action, the onset of subjectively experienced therapeutic effects to treatment is preceded by favourable changes in psychological functioning that can be measured by implicit methods. The aim of this study was to examine additional data to explore this hypothesis in an intention-to-treat repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) study targeting the right parietal cortex. Changes in depression scores from baseline and the sensitivity for recognizing emotional facial expressions were studied in 28 patients with depressive disorder receiving ten sessions of real (n=14) or sham (n=14) rTMS treatments in a double-blind, sham-controlled design. In the patient group results showed significantly higher sensitivity for recognizing angry facial expressions in response to receiving real compared to receiving sham rTMS treatment. Overall mood improvement was similar across real and sham rTMS treatments. However, the sensitivity for recognizing angry facial expressions was correlated to the percentage decrease in depression scores. These results provide the first preliminary support for the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action in rTMS treatment. Received 8 February 2010 ; Reviewed 10 March 2010 ; Revised 17 May 2010 ; Accepted 19 May 2010 ; First published online 29 June 2010 Key words : Angry faces, antidepressant action, depressive disorder, parietal cortex, transcranial magnetic stimulation, treatment.

Introduction According to the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action, the onset of therapeutic effects to treatment is preceded by changes in psychological functioning (Harmer et al. 2009a, Harmer, 2010). Changes in cognitive and emotive processes associated with the neurophysiological Address for correspondence : D. J. L. G. Schutter, Ph.D., Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands. Tel. : +31 30 253 4369 Fax : +31 30 253 4511 Email : [email protected]

action of treatment are usually not consciously accessible to the patient or directly observable by others, but nonetheless they may reveal antidepressant action at an early stage of treatment. Single administrations of the antidepressant agents reboxetine and citalopram have, for instance, been shown to reduce biases for negative information and enhance the processing of positive information without an immediate effect on mood (Harmer et al. 2009b). Similar effects have been observed in non-invasive brain stimulation studies. Single administrations of 1-Hz slowfrequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) have demonstrated decreased attentional

1156

D. J. L. G. Schutter et al.

responses to fearful faces (van Honk et al. 2002), increased attentional responses to angry faces (d’Alfonso et al. 2000) and improved memory performance for happy faces (Schutter & van Honk, 2006). Notably, in all cases, no changes in mood were observed. In the past 15 yr rTMS over the frontal cortex has been explored as an alternative way to treat depressive disorder. Recent meta-analytical studies indicate that rTMS reaches similar antidepressant efficacy as several registered antidepressant drugs (Demitrack & Thase, 2009 ; Schutter, 2009, 2010). Despite being equally effective as antidepressants, the clinical relevance of these effects appears to be moderate. Hence, the need for additional research exploring novel strategies to optimize therapeutic efficacy (Herrmann & Ebmeier, 2006 ; Schutter & van Honk, 2005). Evidence for possible antidepressant working of rTMS applied to the right parietal cortex was found in a sham-controlled study by van Honk et al. (2003). In this study, a significantly increased vigilant response to facial anger accompanied decreases in self-reported depressive mood, following a single 20-min session of 2-Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex in healthy volunteers. The combination of more vigilant responsiveness to facial anger and reduced depressive mood may indicate increased approach-related motivation (Depue & Iacono, 1989 ; Putman et al. 2004). These findings led us to perform a double-blind, shamcontrolled study wherein, for the first time, we explored the antidepressant effects of 10 sessions of 2-Hz rTMS over the right parietal cortex in 34 patients (Schutter et al. 2009). Despite the larger partial antidepressant response observed in the real rTMS condition, we did not find a main effect of treatment on the percentage change in depression scores measured with the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD17). However, we were able to collect and analyse additional behavioural data on the sensitivity for recognizing emotional facial expressions in a subset of 28 patients after the final treatment session, which allowed us to examine the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action in rTMS treatment. In keeping with the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action with data on happy facial expressions (Harmer et al. 2009b ; Schutter & van Honk, 2006) and our previous findings on angry facial expressions (d’Alfonso et al. 2000 ; van Honk et al. 2003) we hypothesized that the antidepressant effects of 2-Hz rTMS over the right parietal cortex would be manifested by increased sensitivity to both angry and happy facial expressions.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics rTMS treatment characteristics

Real (n=14)

Sham (n=14)

p valuea

Age, mean¡S.D., yr Female/Male, n Medication, n Baseline HAMD17d, mean¡S.D.

44¡12 10/4 10 21¡4

46¡13 6/8 9 21¡5

0.69b 0.25c 1.00c 0.93b

a

Two-tailed. Analysis of variance. c Fisher’s exact probability test. d 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. b

Methods Participants Twenty-eight of the thirty-four in-patients and outpatients involved in clinical trial NTR1279 with a primary diagnosis of depressive disorder according to DSM-IV-TR criteria (APA, 2000) and a score of i15 on HAMD17 were included in this follow-up analysis. Current depressive disorder was confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Research Version (SCID-I ; First et al. 1996). Real (n=14) and sham (n=14) treatment groups did not differ in age, sex, medication and baseline HAMD17 scores (Table 1). History of seizures, neurological conditions, metal objects in or around the body that cannot be removed (i.e. cochlear implant, surgical clips, piercings and cardiac pacemaker), heart disease, pregnancy, drug and alcohol abuse were considered exclusion criteria. Antidepressant and hypnotic medication had been stable for at least 6 wk and 2 wk, respectively, prior to the study, and were kept constant during treatment. All participants received written and verbal information on the study and written informed consent was obtained. The study was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the medical ethics committee of Free University Medical Center and St Lucas Andreas Hospital, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Emotional facial recognition task The sensitivity for recognizing facial expressions was measured with a task adapted from Montagne et al. (2005) that used morphed images from neutral face (0 % emotion) to full-blown expression (100 % emotion). Stimuli were taken from four actors (two male, two female) who each posed a facial expression

rTMS treatment and perceptual sensitivity (i.e. neutral, anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) in frontal view. These facial expressions were used to create short video clips that incrementally increase the degree of emotional expression in 10 % steps (e.g. from neutral to anger). First, participants viewed the video clips running from neutral to 20 % expression of all six emotional expressions by all four actors in random order. Next, the same procedure was applied for 30 %, 40 % and so on, until the final sequence of clips in which the neutral face changed into the full-blown expression (100 %). There were 216 trials that were preceded by six practice trials. In each trial participants were required to make a forced choice, without time restriction, between one of six emotional expression labels that were shown on the computer screen. For each emotion, an average sensitivity score was computed on the basis of the minimum degree of emotional expression needed to correctly identify the emotional expression of the four actors with 100 % accuracy. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) TMS was performed using a high-frequency Magpro Dantec magnetic stimulator with a MCF-B65 figureof-eight coil (Medtronic, Denmark). At the start of the first session individual motor threshold (MT) of the right hemisphere was established using the visual twitch method to determine the fixed stimulation intensity (mean¡S.D.) (Pridmore et al. 1998), MTreal [76.2¡16.5 dI/dt (A/s)] and MTsham [84.8¡17.7 dI/dt (A/s)]. Over the course of 2 wk patients received ten 20-min sessions of 2-Hz rTMS at 90 % MT (i.e. 2400 pulses per session). Parameters were selected based on prior research showing that effects of rTMS on cortical excitability stabilize at 1600 pulses (Maeda et al. 2000) and clinical evidence indicating that the magnitude of the effects depends on the number of pulses (Touge et al. 2001). The rationale for applying rTMS at subthreshold intensity is based on previous findings of intensity-dependent increases of brain activity in local and distal sites (Speer et al. 2003). Here, we used lowintensity stimulation in an attempt to keep the effects restricted to the right parietal cortex. These parameter settings have proved successful in a related rTMS study in which reductions in phenomenological, physiological and attentional indices of depressive mood were found (van Honk et al. 2003). Stimulation occurred over the P4 electrode site according to the International 10–20 EEG system targeting the right parietal cortex. The coil was held tangentially to the scalp with the handle pointing to the back and oriented away from the coil’s midline

1157

at 45x. For sham stimulation a specially designed figure-of-eight coil was used. This coil mimics the sensation and sound of real rTMS, but prevents the magnetic field from reaching the target tissue through interception by a built-in permalloy shield plate (MC-P-B70, Medtronic, Denmark). Contact between the physician applying rTMS and the patient was kept to a minimum. Stimulation parameters and procedure were in accordance with safety guidelines (Wassermann, 1998). Procedure Patients were randomly allocated to receive either real or sham rTMS treatment on 10 consecutive working days (i.e. 2 wk) via sealed envelopes opened immediately before the start of the first treatment by the clinician administering rTMS. Prior to entering the study the patient underwent standard clinical, psychiatric and laboratory tests and was screened for contraindications to rTMS. Clinical assessment was repeated after the final treatment session. Depression scores were acquired with HAMD17 at baseline and after the final treatment session. The emotional facial recognition task was administered immediately after the final clinical assessment. Last, patients were asked to indicate whether they had received real or sham treatment. All ratings and tests were performed by trained researchers blind to the actual treatment. Statistical analyses A binomial test was performed to check that patients had remained blind to treatment. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) were performed to test the difference between real and sham rTMS treatments on percentage change from baseline on HAMD17 scores and percentage sensitivity for the recognition of emotional facial expressions. A Pearson’s correlation was performed to examine the relationship between percentage change from baseline on the HAMD17 scores and percentage sensitivity for the recognition of emotional facial expressions. For all tests, a two-tailed significance level of 0.05 (uncorrected) was applied.

Results Treatment was well tolerated by the patients. A binomial test demonstrated that blinding of treatment condition was successful (p>0.5). The percentage reduction from baseline on HAMD17 scores (mean¡ S.E.M) did not differ between real (20¡8.8 %) and sham (8¡7.9 %) rTMS treatments [F(1, 27)=1.00, p=0.33],

1158

D. J. L. G. Schutter et al. 100 90

25 Recognition sensitivity (%)

Reduction in HAMD17 score from baseline (%)

30

20

15

10

5

80 70

*

60 50 40 30 20

0 Sham

Real rTMS treatment

showing that 10 sessions of 2-Hz rTMS over the right parietal cortex did not yield significant improvements on depression severity (cf. Schutter et al. 2009) (Fig. 1). In contrast, significant higher sensitivity (i.e. lower percentage) for the recognition of angry facial expressions was observed in the real (42.3¡4.2 %), compared to the sham (60.7¡6.4 %) rTMS treatments [F(1, 27)=5.76, p=0.02]. The difference between real (39.5¡5.8 %), and sham (29.3¡2.2 %) rTMS treatments for the sensitivity of happy facial expressions did not reach statistical significance [F(1, 27)=2.73, p=0.11]. Sensitivity for the other facial expressions was not statistically different between real and sham rTMS treatments (all p values >0.35) (Fig. 2). Finally, a significant correlation between percentage change from baseline on the HAMD17 scores and sensitivity for angry facial expressions was observed [r(26)=0.51, p=0.005] (Fig. 3), indicating that the percentage reduction on HAMD17 scores from baseline was associated with increased sensitivity for recognition of angry facial expressions. Percentage reduction on HAMD17 scores did not correlate with the sensitivity for the other emotional facial expressions (all p values>0.30).

Discussion The aim of this study was to examine whether the neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action has potential in rTMS treatment. This hypothesis

Disgust Fear Happy Sad Facial expression category

Surprise

Fig. 2. Sensitivity for recognition of emotional facial expressions after 10 sessions of real (%) and sham (&) 2-Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex. * p=0.02 (two-tailed).

100

Reduction in HAMD17 score from baseline (%)

Fig. 1. No significant baseline-corrected change in 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD17) scores following 10 sessions of 2-Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex.

Anger

80 60 40 20 0 –20 –40 –60 –80 20

40 60 80 Anger recognition sensitivity (%)

100

Fig. 3. Percentage reductions on Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD17) scores is associated with increased sensitivity (i.e. lower percentage) for angry facial expressions (n=28). Significantly lower percentages on the recognition of angry facial expressions in the real vs. sham rTMS treatment groups suggest that the sensitivity of angry facial expressions may index changes in psychological function indicative for an antidepressant effect.

states that prior to therapeutic onset positive effects of treatment can already be observed at the information processing level (Harmer et al. 2009a ; Harmer, 2010).

rTMS treatment and perceptual sensitivity Our results showed significantly higher sensitivity in recognizing angry facial expressions following 10 sessions of real vs. sham 2-Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex. Despite observing an overall improvement in mood, no main effect of treatment was found. Furthermore, the sensitivity for recognizing angry facial expressions was correlated to the percentage decrease in HAMD17 scores, indicating that the sensitivity for the angry face may capture aspects of antidepressant action. This latter finding would be in accord with a recent study in elderly depressed patients demonstrating improvements in mood and memory performance for angry facial expressions following antidepressant therapy with escitalopram (Savaskan et al. 2008). As mentioned earlier, heightened sensitivity for angry facial expressions has been interpreted as a behavioural sign of increased approach-related motivation (Putman et al. 2004), a condition which is typically reduced or absent in patients suffering from depressive disorder (Depue & Iacono, 1989). According to the integrative model of mood based on fear and anger traits by Lara et al. (2006), anger is conceptually related to appetitive impulsivity, drive, pleasure, aggressive behaviour, goaldirected behaviour, and dominance. Previous research has shown relationships between reduced cortical responses to anger-evoking events and diminished approach-related motivation as a possible vulnerability factor for depression symptoms (Harmon-Jones et al. 2002). In agreement, a functional magnetic resonance imaging study has shown reduced brain activation in response to angry facial expressions in major depressive disorder (MDD) patients (Lee et al. 2008). Interestingly, the processing of angry facial expressions has been linked to dopaminergic activity (Lawrence et al. 2007), providing an indirect neurochemical link between MDD and anger processing (Dunlop & Nemeroff, 2007). Finally, there is some evidence suggesting that antidepressant medication may have some anger-facilitating effects as paroxetine has been found to promote novelty-seeking in patients with social phobia (Allgulander et al. 1998). Evidence for a role of the parietal cortex in the processing of angry facial expressions and possibly depression was found in an earlier study, which showed that vigilant attention for angry facial expressions was associated with reduced electrocortical activity recorded over the right parietal lobe (Schutter et al. 2001). In agreement, as previously noted in a single-blind, sham-controlled study, we found significant increases in attention to facial anger accompanied decreases in self-reported depressive mood following 2-Hz rTMS

1159

to the right parietal cortex in healthy volunteers (van Honk et al. 2003). The subsequent intentionto-treat double-blind, sham-controlled study in depressed patient did not yield an overall difference in therapeutic response between 10 sessions of real and sham 2-Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex (Schutter et al. 2009). According to some researchers, clinical improvement may not appear until several weeks following treatment onset (Harmer et al. 2009a ; Frazer & Benmansour, 2002). In contrast, recent studies suggest that mood improvements can already be observed within 2 wk of starting of antidepressant treatment (Katz et al. 2010 ; Nakajima et al. 2010). The present data suggest that even though 2 wk of rTMS treatment may be too short to obtain robust and clinically relevant effects, the change in sensitivity for angry faces may, nonetheless, indicate onset of antidepressant action. Even though the precise relationship between the currently observed alteration in perceptual sensitivity and antidepressant action needs to be explored further, the increased sensitivity for angry facial expressions may indicate continuation of treatment. Unfortunately, the hypothesized increase in the sensitivity for happy facial expressions did not reach significance and was not correlated to the baselinecorrected change in HAMD17 scores. There was insufficient statistical power due to the relatively large between-subject variance in the sham condition and a ‘floor ’ effect resulting from the fact that happy facial expressions are already recognized at very low intensities (see Fig. 2). In addition, small sample size and absence of a baseline measure on the recognition sensitivity of emotional facial expressions stress the preliminary nature of the present findings. Finally, despite the sham-controlled design, success of treatment blinding, the time delay between the final treatment and assessment, and the fact that the present findings are in good agreement with other electrophysiological and administration studies (Nakajima et al. 2010 ; Savaskan et al. 2008 ; Schutter et al. 2001), individual differences experienced in site pain or general discomfort as a function of stimulation intensity between real and sham rTMS conditions may have influenced the results. Finally, a general (subconvulsive) modulatory effect on the brain that is independent of the cortical site being targeted may provide another explanation for the present data (Schutter, 2010). In conclusion, increased sensitivity of angry facial expressions after real compared to sham rTMS treatment together with the correlation between the sensitivity of angry facial expressions and percentage

1160

D. J. L. G. Schutter et al.

change in HAMD17 scores is in agreement with previous studies and provides the first evidence in support of the cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of antidepressant action in rTMS treatment. Acknowledgements Dr Dennis J. L. G. Schutter was supported by an Innovational Research Grant (452-07-012) from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). Dr Jack van Honk was supported by a grant from the Hope for Depression Research Foundation (HDRF) and High Potential Grant from Utrecht University, the Netherlands. [The trial ‘Transcranial magnetic stimulation to the right parietal cortex in the treatment of depression ’ is registered at The Netherlands Trial Register (http://www.trialregister. nl/trialreg/admin/rctview.asp?TC=1279).] Statement of Interest None.

References Allgulander C, Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Brandt L (1998). Changes on the temperament and character inventory after paroxetine treatment in volunteers with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychopharmacological Bulletin 34, 165–166. APA (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revision. Washington, DC : American Psychiatric Association. d’Alfonso AAL, van Honk J, Hermans E, Postma A, et al. (2000). Laterality effects in selective attention to threat after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at the prefrontal cortex in female subjects. Neuroscience Letters 280, 195–198. Demitrack MA, Thase ME (2009). Clinical significance of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the treatment of pharmacoresistant depression : synthesis of recent data. Psychopharmacological Bulletin 42, 5–38. Depue RA, Iacono WG (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology 40, 457–492. Dunlop BW, Nemeroff CB (2007). The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 327–337. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders-Research Version (SCID-I, Version 2.0), Biometrics Research Department. New York : New York State Psychiatric Institute. Frazer A, Benmansour S (2002). Delayed pharmacological effects of antidepressants. Molecular Psychiatry 7, S23–S28.

Harmer CJ, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ (2009a). Why do antidepressants take so long to work? a cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. British Journal of Psychiatry 195, 102–108. Harmer CJ, O’Sullivan U, Favaron E, Massey-Chase R, et al. (2009 b). Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 1178–1184. Harmer CJ (2010). Antidepressant drug action : a neuropsychological perspective. Depression and Anxiety 27, 231–233. Herrmann LL, Ebmeier KP (2006). Factors modifying the efficacy of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression : a review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67, 1870–1876. Harmon-Jones E, Abramson LY, Sigelman J, Bohlig A, Hogan ME, Harmon-Jones C (2002). Proneness to hypomania/mania symptoms or depression symptoms and asymmetrical frontal cortical responses to an anger-evoking event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82, 610–618. Katz MM, Bowden CL, Frazer A (2010). Rethinking depression and the actions of antidepressants : uncovering the links between the neural and behavioral elements. Journal of Affective Disorders 120, 16–23. Lara DR, Pinto O, Akiskal K, Akiskal HS (2006). Toward an integrative model of the spectrum of mood, behavioral and personality disorders based on fear and anger traits : I. Clinical implications. Journal of Affective Disorders 94, 67–87. Lawrence AD, Goerendt IK, Brooks DJ (2007). Impaired recognition of facial expressions of anger in parkinson’s disease patients acutely withdrawn from dopamine replacement therapy. Neuropsychologia 45, 65–74. Lee BT, Seok JH, Lee BC, Cho SW, et al. (2008). Neural correlates of affective processing in response to sad and angry facial stimuli in patients with major depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 32, 778–785. Maeda F, Keenan JP, Tormos JM, Topka H, et al. (2000). Interindividual variability of the modulatory effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical excitability. Experimental Brain Research 133, 425–430. Montagne B, Kessels RPC, Frigerio E, de Haan EHF, et al. (2005). Sex differences in the perception of affective facial expressions : do men really lack emotional sensitivity? Cognitive Processing 6, 136–141. Nakajima S, Suzuki T, Watanabe K, Kashima H, et al. (2010). Accelerating response to antidepressant treatment in depression : a review and clinical suggestions. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 34, 259–264. Pridmore S, Fernandes Filho JA, Nahas Z, Liberatos C, et al. (1998). Motor threshold in transcranial magnetic stimulation : a comparison of a neurophysiological method and a visualization of movement method. Journal of ECT 14, 25–27.

rTMS treatment and perceptual sensitivity Putman P, Hermans E, van Honk J (2004). Emotional stroop performance for masked angry faces : it’s BAS, not BIS. Emotion 4, 305–311. Savaskan E, Mu¨ller Bo¨hringer A, Schulz A, et al. (2008). Antidepressive therapy with escitalopram improves mood, cognitive symptoms, and identity memory for angry faces in elderly depressed patients. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 11, 381–388. Schutter DJLG, Putman P, Hermans E, van Honk J (2001). Parietal electroencephalogram beta asymmetry and selective attention to angry facial expressions in healthy human subjects. Neuroscience Letters 314, 13–16. Schutter DJLG, van Honk J (2005). A framework for targeting alternative brain regions with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 30, 91–97. Schutter DJLG, van Honk J (2006). Increased positive emotional memory after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 31, 101–104. Schutter DJLG (2009). Antidepressant efficacy of high-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in double-blind sham controlled designs : a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 39, 65–75. Schutter DJLG (2010). Quantitative review of the efficacy of slow frequency magnetic brain stimulation in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine. Published online : 27 January 2010. doi :10.1017/S003329171000005X. Schutter DJLG, Laman DM, van Honk J, Vergouwen AC, et al. (2009). Partial clinical response to 2 weeks of 2 Hz

1161

repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the right parietal cortex in depression. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 12, 643–650. Speer AM, Willis MW, Herscovitch P, Daube-Witherspoon M, et al. (2003). Intensity-dependent regional cerebral blood flow during 1-Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in healthy volunteers studied with H2 15O positron emission tomography : II. Effects of prefrontal cortex rTMS. Biological Psychiatry 54, 826–832. Touge T, Gerschlager W, Brown P, Rothwell JC (2001). Are the after-effects of low-frequency rTMS on motor cortex excitability due to changes in the efficacy of cortical synapses? Clinical Neurophysiology 112, 2138–2145. van Honk J, Schutter DJLG, d’Alfonso AAL, Kessels RPC, et al. (2002). 1 Hz rTMS over the right prefrontal cortex reduces vigilant attention to unmasked but not to masked fearful faces. Biological Psychiatry 52, 312–317. van Honk J, Schutter DJLG, Putman P, de Haan EHF, et al. (2003). Reductions in phenomenological, physiological and attentional indices of depressive mood after 2 Hz rTMS over the right parietal cortex in healthy human subjects. Psychiatry Research 120, 95–101. Wassermann EM (1998). Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation : report and suggested guidelines from the international workshop on the safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, 5–7 June 1996. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 108, 1–16.