Personality, psychosocial risks at work, and health

0 downloads 0 Views 1MB Size Report
role of personality in the process whereby work-related psychosocial risk factors ...... _____. JOB-RELATED AFFECTIVE WELL-BEING. Houkes et al. (2003). 16.
      

 

 

 

 

 

                                                             

             

Personality, psychosocial risks at work, and health

Katharine Parkes, PhD Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK

A report prepared for “Collège d’expertise sur le suivi statistique des risques psychosociaux au travail”

March 2010

 

Summary This report responds to questions formulated by the Expert Committee about the role of personality in the process whereby work-related psychosocial risk factors are implicated in mental and physical health impairment. There are three main parts to the report. First, as a background to the specific questions asked, models of work stress are described, and the various pathways by which personality and work-related psychosocial factors jointly impact on health, are examined with reference to empirical findings. The second part of the report documents a systematic review of journal articles (published 2000-2009) describing prospective studies that evaluate work-related psychosocial risks and one or more personality measures as predictors of mental and physical health outcomes. A total of 33 studies which met these and other predetermined criteria were identified. Findings from the studies are summarised and reviewed, with particular reference to evidence of the additive, interactive and mediator roles of personality in relation to work-related psychosocial factors and health outcomes. The third section responds to a further question concerned with changes in personality across the life course. Normative age-related changes in mean levels of personality variables are described, and work-related factors associated with individual change are considered, noting evidence of reciprocal influence between work experiences and personality change.

Index

1. Introduction 1.1 Demand-control-support model

1

1.2 Effort-reward imbalance model

1

1.3 Organizational justice model

2

1.4 The role of personality in work stress

2

2. Psychosocial risks, personality and health: A systematic review 2.1 Background

8

2.2 Literature search

10

2.3 Results

12

2.4 Findings for individual personality characteristics

14

2.5 Conclusions

20

3. Personality change over the life course 3.1 Mean-level changes in personality across the life course

41

3.2 Individual differences in patterns of personality change

44

3.3 Implications

47

4. References

48

1   

1. Introduction Evidence from prospective studies shows that exposure to work-related psychosocial risks has an adverse impact on long-term health. Much of this evidence comes from research based on two current models of work stress, the job demand-control-support model (DCS)1 and the Effort-Reward model (ERI)2. These main features of these models, together with a more recent model, the Organizational Justice model3, are outlined below. 1.1

Demand-control-support model

In the DCS model, high demands (e.g. time pressures, work overload), low control (few opportunities to make decisions at work, limited skill utilization), and low social support are predicted to lead to high psycho-physiological strain and, over time, to adverse health outcomes. The significance of the DCS dimensions for health outcomes, particularly cardiovascular disease4-7 and affective well-being8-11 has been widely demonstrated. Significant findings have also been reported for other outcome measures, including minor health complaints12, workability13, absenteeism 14, and suicide/attempted suicide15-17). However, the relative importance of the three DCS dimensions in predicting health outcomes varies across studies. Moreover, concerns have been raised about methodological limitations, particularly in relation to cross-sectional survey studies 8, 18. Relatively little evidence supports the demand x control interaction originally predicted by the job strain model19; a review of ‘high quality’ longitudinal studies concluded that there was only modest support for the interactive hypothesis8. More usually, additive effects are reported20, 21.

1.2

Effort-reward imbalance model

The ERI model proposes that an imbalance between effort (e.g. extrinsic job demands, responsibilities, and obligations) and rewards (money, promotion prospects, job security) is a risk factor for poor health2. Unlike the DCS model, recent versions of the ERI model include an intrinsic personality component, designated ‘over-commitment’ (OC). The nature and hypothesised role of OC has evolved in the development of the ERI model; it now refers to a personality trait combining excessive striving with needs for approval and esteem, and is regarded as a potential moderator variable22. Findings from prospective studies support the ERI model in that high effort coupled with low reward is associated with poor mental and physical (particularly cardiovascular) health 23-25 . Job insecurity has been found to add to the adverse effects of ERI26). However, a recent study questions the value of combining effort and reward into a single measure27, and other evidence suggests that causal relations between ERI and health may be reciprocal rather than unidirectional28. The role of over-commitment has been less widely examined, but it was found to be significant as an additive risk factor, over and above ERI measures, in four out of five studies of CVD incidence, and in five out of eleven studies of CVD symptoms in a review

2   

of ERI research22. However, the review provided little support for the interactive model although a more recent survey, using measures from both the ERI and the DCS models, reported that low control (DCS model) and high OC (ERI model) combined synergistically to give rise to a high levels of depressive symptoms29.

1.3

Organizational justice model

Lack of organizational justice in the workplace has been recognised as a psychosocial risk factor that can lead to adverse mental and physical health outcomes3. The ‘Organizational Justice’ model has two components, procedural injustice (decisions at work lack consistency, openness and input from all affected parties) and relational injustice (lack of considerate and fair treatment of employees by supervisors); these components have been found to predict sleeping problems30, poor mental health31 and cardiovascular mortality32 in prospective studies and to explain variance in health outcomes over and above that accounted for by ERI measures31.

1.4

The role of personality in work stress

The models outlined above do not incorporate individual personality characteristics as predictors of mental and physical health outcomes (with the exception of the OC measure in the ERI model). However, personality is known to be significant in relation to long-term health33-36, and several work stress models include paths among personality traits, objective and perceived work stressors and health outcomes. One such model, the ‘Michigan’ model37-39 has been particularly influential in guiding research into the joint effects of personality variables and psychosocial work stressors, and is used as the basis for the present discussion. As represented in this model, shown in Figure 1.1, objective work characteristics influence subjective perceptions of work stress; these perceptions give rise to short-term affective, cognitive, behavioural and physiological responses which, with continued stressor exposure, lead to chronic long-term health impairment. However, the model also incorporates bi-directional pathways and feedback loops; for instance, long-term health impairment may lead individuals to perceive their work conditions less favourably, or to seek an objectively less demanding job. The influence of individual differences operates at several points in the stress process represented by the Michigan model. For instance, personality traits (and other individual characteristics) may act by influencing selection into different types of job40 and hence exposure to objective stressors, or by influencing work perceptions41, or by directly influencing stress responses and health42, 43. Moreover, the model includes not only direct effects of personality, but also mediating and moderating effects, and bi-directional paths. Personality variables, particularly negative affectivity, are potentially involved in each of these mechanisms44.

MODERATOR VARIABLES: INDIVIDUAL AND SITUATIONAL CHARACTERISTICS Social support Management style

Personality Coping resources

Demographic e.g. age, gender, education

Objective work stressors Work overload Long work hours Paced work, time pressures Lack of control over work tasks Shift work Organizational re-structuring, down-sizing, job insecurity

Behavioural e.g. exercise diet

Short-term responses Perceived work stressors

Genetic e.g. family history of illness

Long-term outcomes

Affective

Medical

Cognitive

Psychological

Behavioural Physiological

e.g. Cardiovascular disease

e.g. Chronic depression Anxiety disorder

Behavioural

e.g. Alcoholism

Figure 1.1 Conceptual model of the stress process Note: Solid lines represent direct effects among variables. Broken lines represent interaction effects. Adapted from Israel et al. (1996) 3

4   

1.4.1

Personality and work stress: Mediating effects

Mediation refers to an indirect process by which the effect of one variable on an outcome measure is transmitted through an intervening variable (see Baron and Cohen45 for statistical methods of testing mediation effects). Thus, personality may influence how individuals perceive the objective work demands to which they are exposed and these perceptions in turn may lead to short-term affective, physiological, and behavioural responses, and eventually to chronic health effects. In this presumed sequence, the effects of personality on long-term health are mediated through perceived work stressors and short-term stress responses. In a longitudinal study of mediation effects using a cross-lagged panel design, Schwarzer et al.46 examined the role of self-efficacy in the process by which work stress led to burnout in teachers. The results showed that self-efficacy was a protective factor; resourceful individuals high in self-efficacy experienced less job stress, which in turn reduced subsequent burnout. Moreover, over the one-year follow-up period, the path from earlier self-efficacy to later burnout was stronger than the (non-significant) reverse causal path. Similarly, a measure of personality resources assessed in childhood and early adulthood was found to predict job satisfaction in middle adulthood, and this relationship was partially mediated by job complexity47. A different form of mediation was reported by Kivimaki et al.48 from a 7 yr follow-up study of hostility and sickness absence in which the personality variable ‘sense of coherence’ (SOC)49 acted as mediator, reducing the association between individual hostility and subsequent sickness absence by 33-50% depending on the outcome measure. In a further study, Feldt et al.50 found that the longitudinal relationship between psychosocial stressors (including job insecurity) and psychosomatic/affective outcomes was mediated by SOC. These two studies suggest that change in SOC may result from exposure to individual and work-related psychosocial factors.

1.4.2

Personality and work stress: Confounding effects

As used in work stress research, the term ‘confounding’ refers to the role of personality traits (or other factors such as demographic or socioeconomic variables) in creating an apparent link between measures of work-related psychosocial risks and health, which may be solely or partly due to a ‘third factor’ effect, the influence of the confounding variable(s) on both perceptions of job characteristics and health. In relation to personality, attention has been focused primarily on neuroticism/NA as a potential confounding factor, i.e. as a source of self-report bias that should be statistically controlled in studies that seek to link perceived job stressors and health outcomes51, 52. Accordingly, self-report studies of psychosocial stressors and strain responses often include NA as a control variable53, 54. If inclusion of NA in a multivariate analysis reduces the relationship between perceived stressors and outcome, then confounding is a possible explanation (although it should be noted that the statistical test for confounding is the same as that for mediation; interpretation depends on the nature of the variables concerned, and the underlying theoretical viewpoint).

5   

The view that NA is simply a ‘nuisance’ variable and a potential source of bias has been disputed by other researchers, particularly, Spector and his colleagues41, 55), who argue that there is little evidence to support a general bias effect that cuts across all stressor and strain variables. They suggest that NA plays an important role in work stress processes which should be addressed rather than statistically controlled, and they describe a series of substantive mechanisms through which NA could affect job stressors and strains, each supported by research evidence41: •



• •





Perception. This mechanism reflects the tendency of individuals high in NA to view the world in a negative light, but their negative self-reports of job stressors are seen as valid indicators of their actual perceptions and experiences. Hyper-responsivity. High NA individuals may respond to the same objective level of stressors more strongly than their low NA counterparts, in which case NA would act as a moderating variable in relations between job stressors and affective responses. Selection. High NA individuals may be in more stressful jobs than those low in NA, because they are recruited for less favourable jobs, or because of self-selection. Stressor creation. High NA individuals may, by their own behaviour, create job stressors for themselves, for example, by creating interpersonal conflicts at work, or by managing their workload less effectively than low NA individuals. Mood. Transitory mood may affect the assessment of NA. If so, and if mood is also affected by job conditions, then a correlation between NA and job stressors could reflect the indirect influence of job stressors on reports of NA, rather than the effects of NA on reports of job stressors. Causality. Exposure to high levels of job stressors may tend to make individuals higher in NA; thus, this mechanism proposes that job characteristics affect the trait level of NA, as assessed empirically.

Whilst these possible mechanisms are discussed in relation to NA by Spector et al.41, they are not necessarily specific to NA; for example, Hoge et al.56 found that sense of coherence had not only direct effects on strain measures, but also showed significant perception, selection, and stressor-creation effects, although work stressors remained substantial predictors of strain.

1.4.3

Personality and work stress: Reciprocal effects and reverse causation

Although theoretical models represent job stressors as having a causal effect on health outcomes, evidence of reciprocal and reverse causal relationships between work-related psychosocial risk factors and health outcomes is increasing. In some instances, also, personality variables are implicated in these reverse or reciprocal relationships. A review of longitudinal field studies of organizational stress, found that about half of the 43 studies evaluated reverse causation in stressor-strain relationships, and some evidence of reversed causal effects was found in about 33% of the studies concerned57. More recently, Dalgard et al.58 found evidence of reverse causation over an 11-yr follow-up period; in this case, it applied to job demands but not to job control.

6   

Several studies have applied structural modelling to longitudinal panel data which allows evaluation not only of direct and reverse causation but also of reciprocal causal effects between exposures and health measures. Although at least one study using structural modelling has affirmed the theoretical causal ordering among DCS job characteristics and work-related psychological well-being59, reciprocal longitudinal findings have also been reported60. Similarly, in an evaluation of cross-lagged reciprocal relationships among job demands, work-home interference, and general health, a reciprocal model proved to be superior to both the direct causation and the reversed causation models61. Structural modelling has also been used to evaluate causal paths between ERI measures and health; using three waves of data collection, Shimazu et al.28 found cross-lagged and causally dominant effects of ERI on employee psychological and physical health, but also evidence of reciprocal effects. Similarly, Xanthopoulou et al.62 found that the best-fit model was one in which not only were personal resources (self-esteem, self-efficacy and optimism) and job resources (e.g. autonomy, support) reciprocally related to work engagement, but there was also a reciprocal relationship between job and personal resources. Taken together, these longitudinal studies do not provide strong support for uni-directional reverse causal paths from strains to stressors although, consistent with the ERI and DCS theoretical models, there is evidence of causal influences from psychosocial stressors to health-related outcomes. However, it is also clear that reciprocal paths play an important role in stressor-strain relationships; moreover, these reciprocal pathways may involve personality variables in addition to measures of psychosocial risk factors and health. Thus, even though personality characteristics are conceptualised to be stable over time, as empirically assessed, they appear to be subject to influence by work conditions over relatively short time periods.

1.4.4

Personality and work stress: Interactive effects

Additive (or main) effects imply that two or more predictor variables contribute independently to explaining variance in an outcome measure; in contrast, interactive (or moderating) effects imply that the magnitude and direction of the effect of one predictor (for example, a psychosocial risk factor) on a outcome measure depends on the level of a second predictor (for example, a personality characteristic). Two forms of interaction can be identified, ‘vulnerability/resilience’ and ‘person-environment fit’. •

Vulnerability implies that a high level of a maladaptive personality trait if combined with a high level of a psychosocial risk factor will result in a disproportionately adverse outcome relative to their additive effects. Thus, for instance, high NA individuals were found to show significantly greater reactivity to high job demands than low NA subjects63. Conversely, resilience or buffering implies that a high level of an adaptive personality resource protects individuals from adverse effects of exposure to psychosocial risk factors.



Person-environment fit. In cross-over interactions, neither high nor low levels of a personality factor are necessarily maladaptive; rather, adverse outcomes arise from

7   

incongruence or ‘lack of fit’ between personality and the environment. For instance, perceived job control and an individual’s locus of control may be congruent or incongruent, and have favorable or unfavorable effects, respectively, on outcomes64.

Although the DCS model does not include personality variables, evidence that personality characteristics may act as moderators of the effects of exposure to high strain (or high isostrain) conditions has been reported. For instance, a significant three-way interaction was found to predict affective well-being in cross-sectional and longitudinal data65; demand and control combined interactively for externals whereas additive effects were found for internals. Other researchers have also found that personality characteristics moderate the effects of DCS dimensions on health outcomes64, 66-68, and have advocated further research to extend the demand/control model with personal characteristics66. The points in the causal process at which personality may moderate work stress effects are not yet clearly established. In particular, moderating variables may influence either or both links in the paths by which objective stressors influence perceptions of work stress, and perceptions of work stress are related to psychological and physiological responses. In particular, little is known about how personality variables may moderate relations between objective and perceived work stressors, although significant dispositional moderators of relations between objective and perceived characteristics of laboratory tasks have been identified69.

8   

2. 2.1

Psychosocial risks, personality and health: A systematic review Background

Over the past five years, prospective research into psychosocial risk factors in relation to physical and mental health outcomes has been the subject of several systematic reviews. In particular, detailed reviews of coronary heart disease (CHD)70; 71, and mental health outcomes, including depression24, 72, 73 in relation to work-related psychosocial exposures have been published. In these reviews, two of which include meta-analyses72, 73, risk factors defined by two theoretical models of work stress, the demand-control-support model (DCS)1 and the effort-reward model (ERI)2 play a major role. A further review, which includes both prospective and cross-sectional findings, focuses on the role of work stress in relation to coronary risk factors (including hypertension, blood lipids, and metabolic syndrome)74. More specifically, Van Vegchel et al22 examine research evidence for the extrinsic (effort reward imbalance) and intrinsic (over-commitment) components of the ERI model in relation to health-related outcomes. In general, and with some significant reservations, these reviews conclude that components of DCS and ERI models are prospective risk factors for heart disease and poor mental health, particularly depression. Indeed, referring to their meta-analytic review of workrelated psychosocial risk factors in relation to mental health outcomes,72 conclude that it provides “robust consistent evidence that (combinations of) high demands and low decision latitude and (combinations of) high efforts and low rewards are prospective risk factors for common mental disorders” (p. 443). Other authors have examined the effects of exposure to psychosocial risks from a wider perspective, considering both individual and environmental factors75-77. In particular, in a systematic review of prospective epidemiological data, Kuper et al.77 concluded that there was evidence for the roles of depression, lack of social support, and psychosocial work characteristics on the aetiology and prognosis of CHD, but that evidence for an effect of anxiety or Type A/hostility was less consistent. In discussing this research, Kuper et al. draw attention to the difficulty of determining the extent of bias in the reporting of psychosocial findings; these authors also note bias may occur after publication in that strongly positive results are more likely to be cited by other papers than weak or negative findings. In the context of the present review, it is relevant that details of the 71 studies of CHD (published up to 2001) summarized by Kuper et al. include almost no references to prospective research into work-related exposures as risk factors for CHD that also includes personality measures. Moreover, in two studies reviewed by Kuper et al that do report both work exposures and personality characteristics, the latter are included only as control variables, the main interest being the role of psychosocial work exposures78, 79. Conversely, prospective studies that focus on individual personality traits as predictors of long-term health outcomes rarely include psychosocial work exposures. Adverse health outcomes, such as all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality, coronary heart disease,

9   

depression and other mental health problems, suicide and attempted suicide, have been linked to aspects of personality in long-term prospective studies. A 2006 review of the literature on the development and course of physical illness highlights the adverse effects of negative affectivity and anger/hostility, and the positive role of optimism33, while more recent studies identify extraversion, conscientiousness, and sense of coherence as predictors of favourable long-term mental and physical health34, 80-82. Possible mechanisms underlying the effects of personality on long-term health include shared genetic influences, health behaviours, and the influence of personality on appraisal, coping, and physiological reactivity; personality may also influence exposure to potential stressors (including those in the workplace) and the stress-reducing resources (e.g. social support) available. One possible explanation of the relative lack of prospective research that combines analysis of personality measures and psychosocial work exposures is that the job strain/iso-strain model1, which has attracted much research attention, only includes work environment characteristics; it does not take into account the possible role of individual differences in personality and coping in responses to adverse work conditions. Proponents of the DCS model point to evidence suggesting that individual differences are unlikely to account for the association between job strain and CHD6, although the need for further research into interactions between environmental stressors and personality characteristics is recognized. An important exception to the general lack of research combining work exposures with personality characteristics is a study based on data from the Whitehall II cohort of UK civil servants5. The analysis included measures of hostility, Type A behaviour, competitiveness, negative affectivity, minor psychiatric disorder, and two coping patterns, together with measures of job control, in relation to newly reported CHD events over a 5.3 yr follow-up period. The results showed that when age-adjusted odds ratios for the effects of low job control on CHD outcomes were compared across sub-groups identified as having or not having each negative personality characteristic, differences were small and not consistently in one direction, nor consistently in the same direction for men and women. These results were not substantially changed by including other control variables, by use of different analyses, or when job demand and social support were used as exposure measures. The authors concluded that adverse effects of low job control could not be explained by confounding effects of negative personal characteristics, or by a generalized tendency for neurotic individuals to complain. They also ruled out possible mediating or moderating effects of negative personality factors, concluding that their findings “seem to justify the relative disregard of personal factors and individual differences concerning low job control” (p. 406). However, the authors noted some psychometric problems with their measures, and suggested that other personal attributes (e.g. locus of control), or use of different work stress models, might produce different results. Moreover, this study only considered CHD outcomes, and the authors emphasised that personal characteristics should certainly not be neglected in the broader field of job stress research. In contrast to the job strain model, the Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) model incorporates an  intrinsic component, a personality characteristic designated over-commitment (OC),

10   

which reflects excessive striving combined with a strong need for approval and esteem. Studies reviewed by Van Vegchel et al.22 suggests that there is evidence for a significant additive role of OC, but there is little support for the model that predicts that OC and ERI act synergistically to predict health outcomes. The aim of the present review is to bring together findings from prospective research which examines personality factors and work-related psychosocial risks as joint predictors of mental and physical health outcomes. The review focuses on two main questions: •

First, is there evidence that personality variables contribute to explaining health outcomes over and above the effects of work-related psychosocial risk factors? In this context, not only direct relationships, but also possible confounding, mediating, and/or reverse or reciprocal effects among personality variables, psychosocial risk factors, and health outcomes are relevant.



Second, is there evidence that personality factors act as moderators of relations between work-related psychosocial risks and health outcomes?

As described below, a systematic search of the literature published from 2000-2009 was undertaken to address these questions.

2.2

Literature search

The main literature search was carried out using the ISI Web of Science, and two components of the ‘Scopus’ database ‘Health Sciences’ (which includes Medline) and ‘Social Sciences & Humanities’ (which covers journals in Psychology and Social Sciences). Additional searches were carried out using OvidSP. Further material was found by examining journal articles listed as having cited the articles located in these searches. The search terms identified prospective/longitudinal studies published in peer-reviewed journals in the years 2000-2009, in which measures of work-related psychosocial risks, including (but not restricted to) dimensions from the Demand-Control-Support (DCS) model and the Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) model, together with one or more personality measures, were used to predict health-related outcomes. Search terms for personality included specific personality variables in addition to the general terms ‘personality’, disposition*, and trait*. The main search terms used are shown in Table 2.1.

2.2.1

Criteria for selecting studies

The many documents located in these searches were examined to identify studies that met pre-determined criteria, formulated with reference to recent systematic reviews of psychosocial risk factors in relation to health outcomes72, 73, 77.

11   

Table 2.1 Main terms used in the literature search Personality

Outcomes

Personality Disposition* Trait* Vulnerable / vulnerability Resilient / resilience Negative affect* Positive affect* Neuroticism Extraversion Optimism Locus of control (LOC) Self-efficacy Sense of coherence (SOC) Hostility Type A Over-commitment (OC) Coping resources NEO inventory NEO-FFI

Mortality Cardiovascular Heart disease Coronary Psychiatric Mental health Distress Anxiety Depression Mood Affect* Psychosomatic Somatic symptoms Well-being Job satisfaction Illness Sickness absence Sick leave Turnover

Psychosocial work environment

Method

(Work* OR job OR occupation*) AND (stress OR psychosocial) AND

Longitudinal* Prospective* Follow-up

Demand* Control Discretion OR decision authority OR decision latitude OR autonomy Social support Iso-strain OR job strain Effort AND reward Organizational injustice Organizational justice Work hours OR time pressure Workload Job security OR job insecurity Re-structuring OR down-sizing OR relocation

Search terms in different groups were joined by AND, and those within groups by OR

The following criteria were applied in selecting studies to be included: •



Longitudinal studies of working age adults which included one or more psychosocial work exposures AND one or more personality characteristics as predictors of health-related outcomes. Outcomes were cardiovascular disease, psychiatric diagnoses, mental and physical health, somatic complaints, insomnia, job-related affective responses, and job-

12   

• • • • • • •

related behavioural outcomes (e.g. absences) identified by hospital or company records, assessed by diagnostic interview, or by referenced self-report scales. Clear descriptions of the measures used. A follow-up period of at least one year Control for baseline level of dependent measure, or exclusion of baseline ‘cases’. A study size of at least 200 participants Sample located in Europe, North America, New Zealand, Australia, Japan or Russia English language articles published in peer-reviewed journals, 2000-2009 Must report statistical results relating to personality variables (i.e. not simply state that they were used as controls)

A total of 33 studies met the criteria and were included in the present review; the relatively small number of studies selected was primarily due to the triple requirement for prospective data in which both personality and psychosocial risk exposures were assessed. As others have noted83, the majority of studies in this research area are either crosssectional and/or do not include measures of both personality and work characteristics.

2.3

Results

Details of the 33 studies included in the review are summarized in Table 2.2. The followup durations generally ranged from 1-10 years, although two studies used childhood measures. Both men and women were included in almost all the studies, but separate results were not always presented; more usually, gender was treated as a control variable. The analysis methods included multiple regression and structural modelling (with continuous variables) and, more frequently, logistic regression models (based on groups with different levels of exposures). The psychosocial exposure measures and types of outcome variables used are outlined below. The personality variables, their main effects on outcome measures, and interactions between personality and psychosocial risks, are then examined in more detail.

2.3.1

Psychosocial work exposures

Almost half the studies reviewed used measure from (or conceptually similar to) the DCS model or the ERI model to assess psychosocial work exposures. DCS model. Ten studies used measures of work-related psychosocial exposures derived directly from the DCS model, or based on similar constructs [6, 7, 8, 10, 12, 16, 19, 21, 32, 33]1. Most of these studies assessed job demand, control, and social support, although in                                                              1  The numbers in square brackets in the text identify the studies summarised in Table 2.2 

13   

some cases only two of the three measures were used. If only one of the three DCS measures was used, the study was included in the ‘other exposures’ group. ERI model. Five studies included in the review were based on the ERI model [1, 2, 4, 9, 31]. All of these studies assessed effort, reward and effort/reward imbalance, although proxy measures were used in some instances, e.g. [31]. Each of these studies also assessed the intrinsic effort component of the ERI model, over-commitment (OC), but only two studies [2, 31] tested the OC x ERI interaction. DCS and ERI models. Three studies [11, 13, 29] used both ERI and DCS measures. Other exposure measures. Non-theoretically based measures were used to assess work exposures in 15 studies; these studies were more heterogeneous than those based on the DCS or ERI models. Some focused specifically on a single exposure measure, assessed subjectively (e.g. job control [15]) or objectively (e.g. unemployment [5]); others used objective and subjective assessments of the same work exposure [3]. In some studies, specific objective work events were assessed in addition to a general perceived work stress measure [e.g. 14]. One study based on the ‘Organizational Justice’ model [27] used measures of relational justice and procedural justice as exposure measures.

2.3.2

Outcome variables

Overall, half the studies used only one outcome measure, although that assessed jobrelated affective responses tended to use several measure, e.g. scales from the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI). Four different types of outcome variables were identified in the studies reviewed. General mental health outcomes [1] to [15]. The mental health outcomes assessed in these studies included general measures of mental health/psychological distress, specific measures of depression and/or anxiety, self-reported health, and insomnia. In most cases, outcomes were assessed by standard self-report scales, but in two studies [6, 7] by interviews carried out either face-to-face or by telephone. Job-related affective well-being [16] to [25]. Six studies in this group used scales from the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) 84, sometimes coupled with more general measures (e.g. fatigue), for assessing outcome [16, 17, 18, 20, 22, 24]. Participants in these studies were primarily teachers, healthcare workers, and other human services personnel, although three studies [18, 20, 25] were based on random samples of the general population (screened to include only employed individuals) or trades unions. The majority of the studies used nontheoretical measures to assess work characteristics, but three studies used measures from the DCS model [16, 19, 21]. Behavioural outcomes [26] to [30]. Of the five studies that used behavioural measures, four derived the outcome data from formal organizational records relating to sickness absence rates [27, 30]; health-care usage [28] and physician visits [29]. Self-reported voluntary job turnover was used as the outcome in the remaining study [26]. One study

14   

[29] included measures from both the DCS and the ERI models, and one used measures of organizational justice [27]; in the remaining three studies, non-theoretically based measures (down-sizing, stressful work events, and work challenge/hindrance stress) were used [26, 28, 30]. Cardiovascular disease outcomes. [31] to [33]. The three studies in this group analysed longitudinal data from the Whitehall II prospective study of UK civil servants; in each case the mean follow-up period was approximately 11-12 yrs. Of the three studies, one study [31] used proxy measures of effort, reward, and OC (single item) to test the ERI model, while two studies [32, 33] used measures from the DCS model of work stress.

2.3.3

Personality variables used in the studies

The majority of the studies assessed only one or two personality characteristics (20 studies reported one personality measure, and 5 studies reported two measures). In all, there were 56 instances in which personality measures were analysed in relation to one or more outcome variables. The personality variables most frequently reported were the OC component of the ERI model and neuroticism/NA, but three other personality measures (hostility/Type A, locus of control/hardiness, and sense of coherence) were each used in at least five of the 33 studies, although not always assessed by the same scales. Of the remaining 11 measures, 7 were used in only one study. Information about the main and interactive effects of personality variables is shown in the summary table (Table 2.2). The statistical data are taken from the fully adjusted models, although several studies noted the problem of possible over-adjustment (e.g. by including as control variables, health behaviours and/or lifestyle variables, which may act as mediators of relations between personality/psychosocial factors, and health outcomes,). Additive effects and, if reported, tests of the moderating effects of personality on relations between psychosocial work exposures and outcomes are shown, together with notes on confounding or mediating effects, or causal path analyses. 12 of the studies reviewed reported testing interactions between personality and work exposures but, in several cases, only limited details of the tests were provided. Findings for each of the main personality measures used are reported below.

2.4

Findings for individual personality characteristics

2.4.1

Over-commitment (OC)

The personality characteristic of over-commitment describes individual attitudes, behaviours and emotions reflecting excessive striving combined with a need for approval and esteem. The measure forms an integral part of the Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) model (Siegrist, 1999). A recent psychometric evaluation demonstrated that the current version of the OC measure and the other ERI scales have stable psychometric properties 85.

15   

Main effects. Eight of the studies included a measure of OC, together with the effort and reward measures from the ERI model; three of these studies also used DCS measures. OC showed significant main effects, additively with ERI measures, in all except one of the studies [29]. Among men, high OC was consistently predictive of adverse health-related outcomes, including psychological ‘caseness’ (GHQ-12), [1], self-reported depression, anxiety, and/or somatic symptoms [2, 4], insomnia [11], poor subjective health ratings [9], and incident CHD [31]. Risk ratios (if reported) were generally in the ‘moderate’ range (1.5 - 2.0), except for CHD for which the HR value for OC was 1.26 (1.09-1.46). Among women, the effects of OC were less consistent; for instance, OC did not predict subjective health ratings among women [9], nor was it a predictor of anxiety or somatic symptoms, although it did predict depression in women [4]. Findings from one study suggested that high OC was a risk factor for professionals, but not for manual workers [13]. Entirely non-significant findings for the main effects of OC were reported in only one study. In this case, separate components of the ERI model (but not OC or effort-reward imbalance) were significant predictors of an objective measure of health care usage [29]. Thus, the strongest results for OC as a psychosocial risk factor were found for men in relation to a range of health-related outcomes, primarily of a psychological or psychosomatic nature. Interactive effects. Although the ERI model includes predicted interactions between OC and effort-reward imbalance, only two out of the eight studies that used the OC measure reported testing OC x ERI interactions. In both cases, the results were non-significant [2, 31]. The present review therefore provides no evidence that OC acts as a moderator of ERI effects in relation to mental health outcomes or incident coronary heart disease, thus contributing further non-significant results to the generally inconsistent evidence reviewed by Van Vegchel et al.22. However, the failure to report tests of interactive effects in six of the studies concerned weakens any wider conclusions that can be drawn. Differences between men and women in the extent to which OC acts as a risk factor for adverse health outcomes suggest that gender x OC interactions might contribute significantly explaining outcome variance.

2.4.2

Neuroticism / negative affectivity (NA)

NA is used here to refer to both neuroticism and negative affectivity, both of which reflect emotional vulnerability, pessimism, and a general disposition to react negatively to life and work stressors; individuals high in NA tend to be anxious, easily upset, often moody or depressed, and focused on negative aspects of self, other people and the world in general. In the studies reviewed, NA was the most frequently reported personality characteristic; it was assessed in 11 of the 33 studies, distributed across all the four types of outcome variables. Five of the studies based on the DCS model and six of the non-theoretically based studies included NA, although none of the ERI studies did so. In six of the studies that used a measure of NA, other aspects of personality were also assessed. NA is often

16   

regarded as a confounding variable in the link between psychosocial work stressors and health outcomes, and several studies noted that it was included for that reason [3, 7, 8]. Main effects. In 9 out the 11 studies (including all the DCS studies), NA was a significant risk factor in multivariate predictive models, particularly in relation to mental health outcomes [3, 7, 8, 14], but also for job-related affective outcomes [16, 23], and for CHD [32, 33]. Risk ratios for NA in relation to mental health outcomes were reported for two studies; the values were 1.98 (1.66–2.36) (men)/1.55(1.33–1.81) (women) [7], and 3.59 (2.06–6.26) (men/women) [14], representing moderate to strong associations. NA was also found to be a significant risk factor for sickness absence in both men and women [27], but it was not related to voluntary job turnover [26]. In the two studies of CHD incidence [32, 33], NA was a main focus of interest rather than being included only as a potential confounder. Adjusted for demographic factors, those in the highest NA tertile (top one-third of scores) were at significant, albeit not large, risk for incident CHD events, hazard ratio=1.32 (1.09 - 1.60); controlling for job strain, health behaviours, and other potential confounders in the multivariate model had little effect on this finding [32]. However, the positive dispositional counterpart of NA, positive affectivity (PA) did not show significant effects, nor did the affect balance score (the difference between PA and NA scores). A further study of NA in relation to CHD [33] (based on a similar Whitehall II dataset) used the ‘Relative Index of Inequality’ (RII) as the risk index. The RII represents the hazard ratio for the extremes of the observed score distribution. In this study the RII value for NA was 1.64 (1.17 – 2.32). In this study NA and inflammatory biomarkers were independent predictors of incident CHD; there was no evidence of mediation effects. Confounding effects of NA. Three studies of mental health outcomes [3, 7, 8] explicitly noted the role of NA as a potential confounder of relations between psychosocial factors and mental health outcomes and reported the significance of NA as a main effect, but only one study included data showing the effects of controlling for NA. In this study [7], the effects of high job demand (highest tertile scores) remained significant with NA in the multivariate model, and the reduction in RR values was small for both men and women. Interactive effects of NA. Two very different studies [16, 27] included tests of interactions between NA and psychosocial risk factors in predicting outcomes; both reported significant findings: Elovainio et al.86 [27] examined the roles of NA and hostility as moderators of relations between measures of organizational justice and sickness absence rates. In addition to a significant main effect of NA in both men and women, NA moderated the effect of relational justice on sickness absence rates among men, but not among women. Thus, consistent with a personality vulnerability model, the combination of a stressful psychosocial context (low relational justice) and high NA among men led to particularly high rates of absence.

17   

Houkes et al.87 [16] used LISREL structural modelling to evaluate additive and interactive effects of NA in a cross-lagged panel design. In the additive model, inclusion of a direct causal path from Time 1 NA to Time 2 emotional exhaustion significantly improved the fit of the synchronous model; however, reverse causal and reciprocal paths were not significant. Interactive effects were evaluated by sub-group analyses; a significant moderating effect of NA on relations between workload and exhaustion was found. The total effect of Time 1 workload on Time 2 emotional exhaustion was .06 in the low NA group and .27 in the high NA group. This analysis confirmed and extended previous findings63, and illustrated one of the substantive NA mechanisms identified by Spector et al.41. In summary, NA was found to act as a significant additive risk factor in almost all the studies in which it was included, but there was little evidence of it acting as a confounding variable. There was also no evidence of reversed causal effects from psychosocial stressors to NA measures, or of reciprocal effects, although these were tested in only one study [16]. Significant interactive effects were found in two studies [16, 27] both of which were consistent with a vulnerability model of NA.

2.4.3

Hostility / Type A behaviour

Main effects of hostility /Type A behaviour. Although both hostility and Type A behaviour pattern have been linked with the incidence of cardiovascular disease88, 89, in the studies reviewed, these personality characteristics were assessed in relation to mental health and behavioural outcomes. Measures of Type A behaviour [20, 28], and hostility [27, 30] were each reported in two studies; in addition, one study [12] assessed both these characteristics, and one study used teacher ratings of hostility at age 8 yrs to predict adult health outcomes in relation to employment status [5]. A significant effect of hostility was found in both the studies that used sickness absence as an outcome measure. In one study, the risk of absence was higher by a factor of 1.2–1.4 among individuals high in hostility than among others [30]. Similarly, the second study reported significant regression coefficients for hostility as a predictor of sickness absence in both men (.15*) and women (.03**) [27]. Hostility, but not Type A, predicted increased depression over a 3-yr follow-up in a large French cohort [12]). Type A was also nonsignificant in relation to health care usage [28], and in an exploratory path analysis in which job demand and workload, and several personality measures, were used to predict fatigue and exhaustion [20]. Confounding/mediating effects of hostility/Type A behaviour. There was no evidence in these studies that hostility or Type A acted as confounding variables in relations between psychosocial factors and health outcomes, or that its effects on outcome measures were mediated by psychosocial factors. Interactive effect of hostility/Type A behaviour pattern. Three studies tested hypothesised interactions between psychosocial risk factors and hostility [5, 27, 30]. In each case, the predicted interactions were significant; in each case, high hostility was a risk factor for

18   

individuals experiencing stressful conditions, specifically, unemployment [5], low job control during organizational down-sizing [30], or lack of procedural justice [27], although in two cases the findings applied only to men [5, 27]. Also, one study tested interactions on an exploratory basis [12]; hostility was a component of one significant interaction. 2.4.4

Locus of control / hardiness

Locus of control (LOC) measures assess the extent to which individuals believe that outcomes are determined by personal effort and ability rather than by external influences such as fate, chance and powerful others90-92. Locus of control, considered together with hardiness (a composite measure in which LOC is the major component93) was used in five studies [6, 15, 20, 21, 28]. These studies reported very diverse findings, specifically, a significant negative main effect of internal LOC on incidence of depression in a large sample of Canadian employees [6]; non-significant results for the main effect of hardiness on health-care usage [28]; moderator effects of LOC on job dissatisfaction, but no additive effects reported [21]; a significant negative effect of hardiness on emotional exhaustion in a longitudinal causal model [20]; and personal control mediating the effect of work control on health over a 10-yr follow-up period [15]. Main effects. Findings from two studies [6, 20] pointed to additive roles for LOC and hardiness, respectively, in attenuating negative affective states associated with psychosocial exposures; in two other studies [15, 21] tests of meditation and moderation rather than possible additive effects were the main focus. Confounding. One study [6] noted that inclusion of personality variables in the regression model reduced the effect of work demand to non-significance; this effect appeared to be due to the effects of two personality variables (locus of control and sense of cohesion) acting either as confounders or mediators. Personal control (LOC) as mediator. Latent growth curve modelling was used to evaluate the roles of work control and personal control (similar to LOC) in predicting health outcomes in a 10-yr follow-up study of men employed in a rural U.S. state94 [15]. The final model demonstrated an indirect influence of work control on the two health outcomes; the effects of initial level of work control and subsequent change in work control on health at follow-up were fully mediated through the corresponding initial level and changes in level of personal control. The findings suggest that personal control can be influenced by work experiences, and that change in personal control has a long-term impact on health, although no reverse causal paths were tested. Moderating effects of locus of control. Significant moderating effects of LOC in relation to exposure measures of the DCS model were demonstrated in one study [21]. In data collected over a 2 yr follow-up period, a significant four-way interaction was found; job demand and control interacted to predict job dissatisfaction only for internal locus of control participants under conditions of high social support. However, the form of the interaction was not readily interpretable, and the interaction term accounted for only a very small proportion of variance in the final model.

19   

2.4.5

Sense of coherence (SOC)

Individuals high in SOC view their environment as manageable and comprehensible; they are more likely to perceive themselves as able to adapt to the demands they experience, less vulnerable to stress, and more able to maintain health49. SOC is highly negatively correlated with NA95, and would therefore be expected to have a positive effect in attenuating or moderating relations between work-related psychosocial risks and health measures. Five of the studies reviewed used a measure of SOC, in relation to measures of general mental health [6, 8, 10] or job-related affective outcomes [24, 25]. Main effects of SOC. Two studies found strong negative relationships of SOC with measures of mental health in multivariate models including psychosocial risk factors and confounding variables [6, 8]. SOC was also included in a causal model of relations between psychosocial work dimensions and four measures of job-related affective wellbeing [25]. No significant path was found between SOC and T2 affective responses (although one reverse causal path from a T1 affective measure to T2 organizational climate was found). SOC as a mediator. In a best-fit structural model, SOC acted as a mediator of the effects of organizational climate and job insecurity on emotional exhaustion and psychosomatic symptoms; also, change in SOC mediate the effect of change in organizational climate on change in both indicators of well-being [24]. SOC as moderator. Moderating effects of SOC were found in one study [10]; under conditions of high job strain, high SOC increased the probability of ‘excellent’ SRH ratings (OR=2.35**), and reduced the probability of musculoskeletal pain (OR=.34**).

2.4.6

Other personality variables

The main findings for other personality variables used in the studies reviewed are summarised below. •



• •



Four studies included extraversion [14, 20, 23, 26] but it was significant only in relation to voluntary job turnover [26]. Positive affectivity (PA), which is closely linked to extraversion, was non-significant in relation to incident CHD [32]. Low self-esteem was a risk factor for increase in depression [12]; it was also negatively related to mental distress and emotional exhaustion in men but not women [18]. It was non-significant in two other studies [6, 19]. Conscientiousness was used in two studies [20, 26,] as part of the Five-Factor Personality Inventory96. It was not significant in either study. Self-efficacy was also used in two studies. In one study [22] it was a significant predictor of burnout, the path being mediated by work stress. In the other study [17], it was a direct predictor of client engagement (the reverse of burnout). Other personality measures, including optimism (significantly negatively related to job-related affective distress among men [18]), and openness (non-significant in relation to onset of depression [14]) were each used only in a single study.

20   

2.5

Conclusions

The findings outlined above demonstrate that personality is implicated in several different ways in work stress processes. Thus, the effects of work-related psychosocial stressors on health may be independent of personality; they may be confounded by personality; they may be moderated by personality; or they may mediate the effects of personality. These and other more complex relationships (including reverse causation and reciprocal effects) were potentially within the scope of the present review. However, although additive effects were tested in almost every study, some important mechanisms were evaluated in relatively few of the studies concerned. In particular, in spite of the potential importance of moderator effects in stress processes, only 12 out of the 33 studies (36.4%) reported tests of interactions between psychosocial stressors and personality. This proportion agrees closely with findings from a review of multivariate analyses in 56 papers published in 2002, in which self-reported health was the outcome variable and which met other specified criteria97. Failure to report tests of interactions was the most common problem identified; it applied to 63% of the studies examined, a proportion virtually identical to that found in the present review. Thus, almost two thirds of the studies considered here did not test any hypotheses predicting interactions between personality and psychosocial stressors, or even evaluate possible interactions on an exploratory basis (or, if such tests were carried out, they were not reported). In the majority of the studies reviewed, therefore, the idea that personality characteristics may make individuals more vulnerable to, or more resistant to, the adverse effects of psychosocial stressors was apparently not addressed. This finding is surprising as several of the personality variables concerned (e.g. NA, locus of control, sense of coherence) have been shown to act as moderators of relations between psychosocial stressors and health outcomes44, 63, 65, 98, 99. Failure to consider possible interactions can lead to potentially significant and revealing moderating effects remaining concealed, while weak and/or non-significant main effects are reported. This problem is especially relevant in relation to ‘cross-over’ interactions in which the main effects of both interacting variables may be non-significant. It is also noteworthy that in the relatively few studies in which moderating effects were reported, the majority (75%) found significant results for one or more interactions. It is possible that this finding reflects a reporting bias, that is, the tendency for significant findings to be reported while non-significant findings are disregarded. Moreover, two of the three studies which reported non-significant interactions were based on the ERI model in which OC, regarded as a personality moderator variable, is an inherent component, thus making it more likely that interactions will be tested and reported, even if non-significant. An additional problem in drawing general conclusions from the present review was that the 33 studies that met the selection criteria proved to be very heterogeneous, ranging from epidemiological research primarily concerned with identifying psychosocial risk factors in large population samples to studies that used structural models to evaluate the magnitude

21   

and direction of causal pathways among measures of personality, psychosocial stressors, and health in particular employee groups. A particular feature of epidemiological research was the tendency to dichotomise (or otherwise sub-divide) continuous variables, thus losing information from the original scales. Also, epidemiological studies were more likely to present separate data analyses for men and women, rather than treat gender, and interactions with gender, as factors in a multivariate model. Although all the studies were prospective with initial baseline data collection and one or more follow-up assessments, they also varied widely in the nature and size of the samples, the follow-up duration, the number of waves of data collection, the psychosocial measures used, and the types of outcomes assessed. These differences in research aims and methodology limit direct comparisons across studies. Instead, as a framework for drawing together the main findings from the review as a whole, a series of general questions is addressed below with reference to the findings of the papers reviewed.

2.5.1

Do personality characteristics act as independent risk factors?

This question addresses the issue of the independent additive effects of personality and work-related psychosocial factors on mental and physical health. Two personality variables identified in the present review, over-commitment (OC) and neuroticism (NA) stood out as being significant, consistent, and moderately strong risk factors for adverse mental health and job-related affective outcomes, and significant (although smaller) risk factors for CHD incidence. NA was a significant predictor in 9 of the 11 studies in which it was assessed, while OC was significant in 7 out of 8 studies. Independently of psychosocial exposures, NA was a significant predictor not only for selfreported mental health, job-related distress and incident CHD but also, among women, for objectively recorded sickness absences. Thus, as a risk factor, the independent effects of NA were apparent in relation to psychosocial risk factors represented in two different theoretical models, and a range of non-theoretically based measures, and in relation to each different type of health outcome. The finding that NA is associated with a range of adverse health effects in a variety of different contexts, is consistent with evidence summarised in a recent review article100. This article describes the many mental and physical health disorders, the impaired quality of life, and the high usage of health care services, associated with high NA. The author concludes that there is “growing evidence that neuroticism is a psychological trait of profound public health significance”100, and recommends that understanding the nature and origins of neuroticism, and the mechanisms which link it to mental and physical disorders, should be a research priority. Whilst the findings for other personality variables were less clear than those for NA and OC, three other measures (hostility/Type A, locus of control, and sense of coherence), each used in at least five of the studies reviewed, also contributed additively to multivariate models. In particular, ‘sense of coherence’ which is conceptualised as a positive

22   

personality resource associated with adaptive coping behaviours, was a significant predictor of favourable health outcomes in two studies. Hostility showed significant and adverse main effects in four studies, but it also interacted with psychosocial work dimensions to predict outcomes.

2.5.2 Do personality measures act as confounding variables? There was little evidence to suggest that personality variables acted as confounders of relations between work-related psychosocial factors and health outcomes. In one study, control for personality variables reduced the effects of work demand to non-significance, but other studies specifically noted the absence of personality confounding effects. It is relevant that the studies reviewed included control for the baseline level of the dependent variable (and some screened baseline ‘cases’ out of the analysis sample). Consequently, variance associated with personality and other potential confounding factors would be largely accounted for by the dependent variable as assessed at baseline, thereby reducing potential confounding effects in longitudinal analyses. Thus, although NA tends to act as a confounding variable in cross-sectional studies of psychosocial stressors, particularly in relation to self-reported health outcomes101, in the prospective studies reviewed here, confounding effects of NA were not markedly apparent.

2.5.2 Does personality moderate relations between psychosocial stressors and health? In the 12 studies that tested interactions between personality and psychosocial stressors, hostility stood out as showing the most consistent moderating effects. Thus, in significant interactions, hostility was a risk factor for poor self-rated health among unemployed but not employed men; it interacted with procedural justice to predict sickness absence, but only among men; and, during a period of down-sizing, it interacted with job control, also to predict sickness absence. In addition, in an exploratory test of multiple interactions, hostility interacted with job control; high-hostile men appeared to benefit more from high job control (in terms of reduced depression) than low-hostile men. These findings highlight the particularly adverse effects observed when high hostile individuals are exposed to stressful work conditions, including job insecurity or actual job loss, lack of procedural justice at work (i.e. lack of fairness in organizational policies and practices), and low control at work. It is also noteworthy that the present review included only two studies in which sickness absence was the outcome measure, and in both these studies, hostility moderated the effect of work conditions on absence. It is possible that high hostility leads to lack of work-related social support, and difficult interpersonal relations with co-workers, accentuating negative responses to stressful conditions, including withdrawal from the work situation. NA was found to be a significant moderating factor in two studies, both of which showed that high NA individuals were more vulnerable to work-related stressors (high workload in

23   

one study and, in the other, lack of relational justice, among men). Although interactions were also reported from other studies (e.g. SOC moderated the effects of high strain conditions on health outcomes; LOC moderated the effects of iso-strain on job dissatisfaction; and self-efficacy moderated the effects of role ambiguity and workload on job satisfaction), only for NA and hostility was there evidence from more than one study of significant findings for predicted interactive effects. The small number of studies that tested interactions precluded identification of particular psychosocial stressors and personality variables that combine synergistically to predict specific types of outcomes. The practical implication of interactions between psychosocial risk factors and personality, as noted by Houkes et al.87, is that organizations wishing to reduce ill-health and distress among employees should consider both work characteristics and individual factors. A similar conclusion was reached by Ferguson et al.83 from meta-analytic structural equation modelling of ‘perceived negative job characteristics’ (PNJC) and ‘negatively oriented personality’ (NOP) as predictors of concurrent and future symptom reporting. In this metaanalysis, it was found that a model based solely on NOP offered a more parsimonious account of baseline and future symptom reporting than did PNJC. In view of the importance of NOP, the authors concluded that interventions should focus on individuals and organizations, rather than solely targeting interventions at the organizational level.

2.5.3

Are the effects of personality mediated through psychosocial stressors?

Three studies examined mediating effects using structural modelling analyses. In one of these studies, a general measure of work stress was found to mediate the path between the personality measure of self-efficacy and the health measure of burnout46; thus, in this model, self-efficacy influenced perceptions of work stress which in turn influenced burnout. This path represents the ‘normal’ theoretically predicted causal direction, in which a personality trait influences work perceptions which in turn influence health. There was no evidence of reverse causation in this analysis. In contrast, no support for the ‘normal’ causal direction was reported by Mäkikangas et al.102; rather one reverse causal path was found in which affective response at follow-up predicted baseline organizational climate. Also, in this study, SOC did not act as a mediator. Two other studies of mediation adopted a different model, in which the personality variable was treated as the mediator between psychosocial stressors and health. First, over a one-year period, a good organizational climate and low job insecurity at TI was positively related to high levels of SOC, which in turn predicted affective well-being at Time 250; in this study, also, changes in organizational climate were related to changes in well-being through changes in SOC. Second, also using latent growth curve modelling, the effects of level and change in job control on health measures over a ten-year period were found to be mediated by level and change in personal control (a measure of LOC)94. Both these studies demonstrated that personality measures changed in response to changing work conditions. Whilst measures of personality would normally be regarded as relatively stable, individual levels of SOC49 are considered to be malleable and shaped not

24   

only by early childhood but also by adult work and life experiences50. The role of SOC as a mediator variable is consistent with this interpretation. In this case, work experiences gave rise to change in SOC over a time period of only one year, which accords with findings suggesting that the SOC measure has a significant state component, rather than representing a stable global orientation103. Evidence that personal control (LOC) acted as a mediator variable related to a longer time period (10 yrs)94 but similar arguments apply, and are consistent with earlier findings suggesting that LOC is reciprocally related to work conditions104, 105.

2.5.4

Implications

Several general conclusions can be drawn from the results outlined above. First, it is clear that personality plays significant roles in the paths by which work-related psychosocial stressors are associated with adverse mental and physical health outcomes, although findings across different studies are not entirely consistent. Second, these paths are not necessarily in the theoretically-predicted directions, nor are they necessarily unidirectional, although the relatively small sample of studies reviewed does not allow clear conclusions about the relative importance of the different mechanisms. Third, the findings tentatively suggest that different personality traits may act through different pathways. For instance, negative affectivity and over-commitment showed strong additive effects, largely independent of work-related psychosocial risks, hostility acted as a vulnerability factor interacting with psychosocial risks, and mediator or mediated effects were most apparent for the dimensions of locus of control and sense of coherence. However, any such conclusions must be qualified by the fact that in many studies only additive effects were considered, and thus more complex mechanisms may have been overlooked. The relevance of examining the various mechanisms by which personality is implicated in work stress processes depends on the focus, aims and methods of any particular study. However, it is clear that the importance of possible interactive effects has been overlooked in much research. In particular, interactions reveal ways in which a particular personality trait coupled with a particular psychosocial work exposure may lead to poor mental or physical health while either factor alone would not have done so. Such information can potentially guide more effective intervention strategies to enhance well-being at work. From both research and applied viewpoints, it would be valuable if future research gave greater attention to predicting and testing interactive effects, using appropriate statistical methods, and reporting the findings irrespective of whether or not they are significant. Prospective studies of mediating effects, and particularly the possible role of psychosocial work exposures in bringing about changes in personality, would also contribute to a better understanding of work stress processes. Issues such as these have been widely recognized in the research literature. For instance, it has been suggested that one reason the success of organisational attempts to modify job strain have so far been relatively modest may be that the effects of personality are largely ignored when work stress is considered to originate mainly from work106. Similarly,

25   

Wilhelm et al.107 note the need for “more longitudinal studies and consideration of factors which the worker brings to the workplace (psychosocial issues, personality traits), as well as interpersonal issues and consideration of systemic, organizational, political and economic factors, including leadership styles”. Others have also recommended that more attention be given to personality in work stress research98, 108-110. More generally, over the past two decades, industrial changes (including organizational restructuring, down-sizing, privatisation, out-sourcing and contract work) associated with economic globalisation have increased job insecurity and led to changes in work processes and management behaviour. Such changes may adversely affect employees; for instance, in a recent survey, 85% of the 86 studies examined reported adverse health and safety effects of down-sizing111. These changes in work conditions have led to recognition of the need to expand traditional work stress models to include new work dimensions, such as employment uncertainty, irregular/atypical schedules, emotional work demands, and the quality of social relationships at work112. New models which incorporate not only novel work dimensions but also take into account the role of personality in coping and adaptation are needed to fully address these concerns.

26

Table 2.2 Summary table of the 33 studies reviewed Author

Sample

Follow-up duration

Dependent measure (s)

Control for initial/ baseline level of dependent measure(s)

Exposure measure(s)

Personality measures

Confounding /control variables included in analysis model

Main effects of personality

Tests of personality x psychosocial work factors interactions reported ?

Significant interactions between personality x work factors ☺☺ indicates initial hypotheses predicted specific personality x work moderating effects ☺ Exploratory analyses of moderating effects

MENTAL HEALTH OUTCOMES Bridger et al (2009)

791 UK naval personnel 319 females 472 males

1 yr 3 waves of data collection

Strain defined as GHQ-12 ‘caseness’, cut point ≥ 4

Chronic strain group (‘cases’ at each assessment, n=78), compared with ‘strainfree’ group (n=345)

Effort/reward (ERI measures)

Overcommitment (OC)

Anxiety and depression (HADS scales) Life satisfaction

Yes

Work hours

Overcommitment (OC)

1 BuddebergFischer et al (2008)

433 Swiss medical residents (physicians)

2 yrs (T2 and T3 from prospective study)

2 Burgard et al (2009) Good study

3

Two samples of employed U.S. adults N= 1507 age 25+ yr 1986-1989 N=1216, age 25-74 yrs 1995-2005

3 / 10 yrs 2 waves of data collection

ERI measures of effort and reward

Physical/mental well-being Self-rated health (SRH) Depressive symptoms Telephone interview assessments

Yes

Perceived job insecurity Objective job insecurity (actual experience of unemployment for one yr prior to each data collection wave

Neuroticism

Over-commitment: (β=.32***), and ERI (β=.19*) were both significant in the logistic model.

Gender Self-reported health at baseline

Gender, age, race, employment history, education, income, blood pressure, etc

Significant additive effects of ERI and OC on health outcomes (p