White matter integrity is associated with cognitive ... - Semantic Scholar

0 downloads 0 Views 198KB Size Report
(FA), and speed of cognitive processing using tract-based ... The Author(s) 2012. ..... Fry AF, Hale S. Processing speed, working memory and fluid intelli-.
Neuro-Oncology 14(9):1185 –1193, 2012. doi:10.1093/neuonc/nos154

N E U RO - O N CO LO GY

White matter integrity is associated with cognitive processing in patients treated for a posterior fossa brain tumor Shawna L. Palmer, John O. Glass, Yimei Li, Robert Ogg, Ibrahim Qaddoumi, Gregory T. Armstrong, Karen Wright, Cynthia Wetmore, Alberto Broniscer, Amar Gajjar, and Wilburn E. Reddick Department of Psychology (S.L.P.); Department of Radiological Sciences (J.O.G., R.O., W.E.R.); Department of Biostatistics (Y.L.); Department of Oncology (I.Q., K.W., C.W., A.B., A.G.); Department of Epidemiology and Cancer Control (G.T.A.), St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital, Memphis, Tennessee

Children treated for posterior fossa tumors experience reduced cognitive processing speed and, after imaging, show damage to white matter (WM) tracts in the brain. This study explores relationships between white matter microstructure, assessed by fractional anisotropy (FA), and speed of cognitive processing using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS). At 36 months after treatment with radiotherapy and chemotherapy, 40 patients completed an MRI examination and neuropsychological evaluation. Patients were matched with healthy control subjects based on age, sex, and race. Individual FA values were extracted from examinations for all voxels identified as having significant association between processing speed and FA using TBSS. The regions were labeled anatomically, and fiber tracts were grouped into larger fiber bundle categories based on their anatomical and functional associations. Analyses were performed between mean skeletal FA values in each of the fiber bundles and each of the cognitive processing scores controlling for age. Children 3 years after treatment for posterior fossa brain tumors demonstrate significantly lower processing speed associated with decreased FA, compared with their healthy peers. Commissural fibers in the corpus callosum were negatively affected by disease and therapy with detrimental consequence on patients’ cognitive processing. Diffusion tensor imaging of the white matter tracts in the brain is relevant to determining potential mechanisms underlying clinically meaningful change in

Received April 18, 2012; accepted June 22, 2012. Corresponding Author: Shawna L. Palmer, PhD, St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital, Department of Psychology, Mail Stop 740, 262 Danny Thomas Place, Memphis, TN 38105 (shawna.palmer@stjude. org).

cognitive performance. Neuroprotective strategies are needed to preserve critical functions. Keywords: diffusion tensor imaging, FA, medulloblastoma, processing speed, TBSS.

M

edulloblastoma occurs in the posterior fossa and is the most common form of malignant brain tumor diagnosed in children. Pediatric medulloblastoma can occur at any age but is typically diagnosed in children up to 10 years of age, with a peak incidence at 5 years of age.1,2 Because of the tumor’s high predilection to spread in the neuraxis, contemporary treatment protocols include maximal surgical resection, radiation therapy to the entire neuraxis and tumor bed in children ≥3 years of age, and adjuvant chemotherapy. Treatment is risk-adapted with those patients considered to be at high risk receiving higher doses of radiation therapy.3 Rates of survival have been steadily increasing. A recent report shows an overall 5-year survival rate of 85% for those patients with average-risk medulloblastoma and 70% for those with high-risk disease.3 However, successful treatment has been associated with an unacceptable number of long-term limitations in regard to function, especially as it relates to cognitive performance.4 – 7 An idea maintained in previous publications,8 – 10 slowed information processing speed may be the first cognitive deficit to emerge following treatment. The ability to process information efficiently normally increases rapidly at an early age and, among healthy children, continues to improve throughout childhood, eventually reaching adult levels of performance during late adolescence.11 Increases in knowledge and skill acquisition are associated with increases in processing speed. In contrast, children who have been treated for

# The Author(s) 2012. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Neuro-Oncology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

brain tumors have shown a decreased rate of knowledge and skill acquisition over time from diagnosis, leading to a decline in overall intellect.9,12 Deficits have also been found to be more prominent for those children who receive a diagnosis and are treated at a younger age.13 Recent studies have included diagnostic neuroimaging to supplement behavioral studies examining longterm impact of treatment, with results suggesting that this impaired speed of information processing may result from damage to the white matter (WM) structures of the brain.8,14,15 Myelination allows for faster conduction of electrical impulses,16 and unlike the unmyelinated gray matter that develops rapidly by 4 years of age, the process of myelination in the WM continues into the second decade of life.17 Therefore, WM maturity is directly related to age of the patient. Consequently, WM has been found to be extremely vulnerable to injury from cranial radiation and chemotherapy, especially during periods of rapid myelination.4,18 WM volumes among children treated for medulloblastoma have been found to be reduced, compared with patients who have not received radiation.19,20 A subsequent longitudinal study14 found that, after treatment, both older and younger patients lost WM volume at a similar rate. However, because younger children begin treatment with a lower volume of mature WM, they experience greater deviation from expected developmental norms.15 In addition, studies of radiation dose-tissue response have shown that patients who received reduced doses had a slower rate of WM loss, compared with those who received the higher doses.14 This dose-tissue response among patients with medulloblastoma was also supported by Palmer et al., who found significant volume decreases in the posterior regions of the corpus callosum, the area nearest the tumor bed and receiving the highest doses of radiation.21 Thus, patients who are older at the time of treatment and, therefore, more neurologically mature may be better able to compensate for the detrimental effects of cranial radiation. Although extremely valuable, quantitative volumetric imaging methods cannot detect changes to WM microstructure. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is a magnetic resonance technique that allows researchers to examine the integrity of the WM tissue, measured as fractional anisotropy (FA). Normally, as a child ages, FA increases in a nonlinear pattern, corresponding to increasing axon myelination. In addition, the association between FA in specific brain regions and the development of cognitive functions has been established among healthy children and specific medical populations.22,23 While assessing children with traumatic brain injuries, researchers found that those with lower FA values in the corpus callosum also exhibited slower processing speed and reaction times.23,24 WM FA was also found to be associated with processing and motor speed in a small group of young cancer survivors.25 Previous studies have clearly shown that children treated for posterior fossa tumors are susceptible to WM damage and vulnerable to decreases in the ability

1186

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

to process information efficiently. Therefore using DTI and tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS), we explored how WM integrity may influence cognitive processing in children treated for posterior fossa tumors, compared with carefully matched, healthy control subjects.

Materials and Methods Patients Study participants were enrolled on an institutional review board –approved clinical trial for patients with newly diagnosed medulloblastoma or atypical teratoid rhabdoid tumor. Written consent was obtained for participation. Patients who were at least 3 years from diagnosis were eligible for the study (n ¼ 66). At 36 months after diagnosis, patients must have completed an MRI examination, including DTI sequences, and a valid neuropsychological evaluation, including an assessment of information processing speed. Patients were excluded because of metallic artifacts (n ¼ 2), missing DTI (n ¼ 4), or poor quality on MRI restricting normalization (n ¼ 3). Additional exclusion criteria included language barriers restricting valid neuropsychology evaluation (n ¼ 2), poor physical well-being restricting valid assessment during their scheduled visit (n ¼ 3), progressive disease for which the patient was taken off study (n ¼ 3), pre-existing psychological deficits (n ¼ 1), parent refusal (n ¼ 6), and completing a partial neuropsychological evaluation that did not include information processing speed (n ¼ 1). One additional patient was excluded because of severe residual posterior fossa syndrome. Forty patients (27 males and 13 females) ranging in age from 3.1 to 20.3 years at diagnosis (Mean ¼ 9.9 years, SD ¼ 4.3), and from 6.0 to 23.5 years of age at time of evaluation (Mean ¼ 12.8, SD ¼ 4.4), were included in the study group. According to enrollment documents, 33 patients were white, 3 were African American, 2 were Asian, and 2 were classified as “other.” All patients underwent maximal surgical resection, and 2 of the 40 patients received a diagnosis of postsurgery posterior fossa syndrome that resolved prior to the chemotherapy phase of treatment. All patients were treated with risk-adapted craniospinal irradiation (CSI), followed by 4 cycles of high-dose chemotherapy (cyclophosphamide, cisplatin, and vincristine) with stem cell support. High-risk (HR, n ¼ 7) patients received 36 – 39.6 Gy CSI and a 3-D conformal boost of 55.8– 59.4 Gy to the primary site. The HR patients included 6 who received a diagnosis of medulloblastoma and 1 patient with atypical teratoid rhabdoid tumor. Average-risk (AR, n ¼ 33) patients received 23.4 Gy CSI and a 3-D conformal boost of 55.8 Gy to the primary site. Thirty-two patients with medulloblastoma and 1 with atypical teratoid rhabdoid tumor comprised the AR group.

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

Healthy Controls Control participants were enrolled on an institutional review board – approved clinical trial examining neural substrates of reading. Written consent was obtained for participation. Those who were at least 6 years of age were eligible for the study (n ¼ 93). Each control subject completed an MRI examination with DTI sequences and a valid neuropsychological evaluation, including an assessment of information processing speed. Forty control subjects were matched with corresponding patients based on age at the time of the MRI. Control subjects were an average of 12.86 years of age (range, 6.03 –23.68 years). Thirty-nine pairs were also matched according to sex (n ¼ 26 males) and 35 were matched according to race (n ¼ 36 white, n ¼ 4 African American). Assessment of Information Processing Speed All participants completed a protocol-driven evaluation of cognitive function using the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities (Third Edition).26 The Woodcock-Johnson battery was extensively normalized, using .8800 healthy subjects in .100 geographically diverse US communities. Two subtests designed to measure speed of information processing were of particular interest: (i) decision speed developed to test processing of semantic information and (ii) visual matching developed to test speed of processing visual perceptual information. Together, these 2 subtests combine to produce the processing speed composite score, a standardized measure of overall processing speed. Age-adjusted standard scores have a population mean of 100 and a SD of 15. Diffusion Tensor Imaging MR Acquisition.—Diffusion tensor imaging was acquired on one of two 1.5 Tesla Avanto MR scanners (Siemens Medical Systems) using bipolar diffusionencoding gradients to reduce gradient-induced eddy currents that cause image distortion and degradation.27,28 All images were acquired using a double-spin echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence (TR/TE ¼ 10/100 ms, b ¼ 1000 ms). Imaging sets were acquired as forty 3-mm thick contiguous axial sections with whole-head coverage, 128 square matrix, and 22-cm field-of-view (acquired resolution of 1.7 × 1.7 × 3.0 mm). Four acquisitions were acquired with 12 noncolinear, noncoplanar diffusion gradient directions to calculate the diffusion tensor for each voxel and to ensure the highest signal to noise possible in a limited amount of time to minimize the risk-benefit ratio for this young vulnerable population. DTI Analysis.—Voxel-wise tensor calculations were performed with the DTI toolkit under SPM8 (http:// www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Data from the 4 acquisitions were realigned before tensor calculation to

correct for linear image drift, and the mean of the 4 realigned image sets were used for tensor calculation. after the tensors were calculated, Eigen-values were derived to calculate an FA map for the whole brain. Tract-Based Analyses.—The quantitative DTI maps of FA were processed following the TBSS pipeline, part of FMRIB Software Library (FSL, http://www.fmrib.ox. ac.uk/fsl). TBSS is more reliable than other techniques because it minimizes inter-subject variability. TBSS registration non-linearly transformed the FA images into a standard space using the FMRIB58_FA image as the target. After registration, the tract skeletonization process was performed using an FA lower threshold of 0.25. The final WM skeleton represented the fiber bundle centers across both patients and control subjects. FA skeletons were compared between patients with medulloblastoma and control subjects with use of the randomize permutation algorithm in FSL to perform multiple regression analyses.29 Voxel-wise tests were performed with 5000 permutations using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) while treating age as a covariate. Multiple comparisons were accounted for by controlling for family-wise error rates. The first analysis performed was a simple group-wise Student’s t test of FA values between patients and control subjects. The contrast of interest was the FA of voxels, which were greater in control subjects than in patients. Additional analyses performed were conducted using the following multiple linear regression, FAi = b0 + b1 groupi + b2 PSi + b3 (groupi × PSi ) + b4 agei + 1i , where i ¼ 1, . . . , 80 and 1i is a random error. In this equation, the group variable was set to one for patients and zero for control subjects. Age was defined as the age of the subject at examination, and processing speed (PS) was the overall speed of processing standard cluster score for each subject. The second analysis evaluated voxels in which PS was significantly associated with FA including the entire subject pool, both patients and control subjects. The third analysis identified voxels that were significantly different between patients and control subjects in their FA association with overall speed of processing standard cluster score. Only fully corrected P values ,.01 generated by 5000 permutations were considered to be statistically significant. The identified regions were labeled anatomically using the JHU-ICBM-DTI-81 WM atlas.30 Fiber tracts were grouped according to anatomical and functional associations with larger fiber bundles.31 Statistical Approach.—All statistical analyses were conducted using a statistical software package (SPSS for Windows, version 15.0, 2006; SPSS). Speed of information processing was examined for all participants. Measures of PS were also examined among the patient group with regard to risk (AR vs HR) and age at the time of diagnosis. Paired sample t tests were conducted

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

1187

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

to compare patients and control subjects on visual matching, decision speed, and overall processing speed. Paired sample t tests were also conducted to compare FA in each identified fiber tract. The observed WM FA in each fiber bundle demonstrated a positive relationship with age of the patient. Therefore, post hoc regression analyses were completed to remove age-related variance. Separate models were developed for each fiber bundle to determine whether the relationship between FA and processing speed remained significant after accounting for age. Because of the exploratory nature of the study, no adjustment was made for multiple comparisons.

Results Processing Speed Patients.—Overall PS was found to be in the low average range for the patient group (Mean ¼ 85.6, SD ¼ 15.0). Visual matching was also in the low average range (Mean ¼ 82.2, SD ¼ 14.9), whereas decision speed was in the average range (Mean ¼ 92.5, SD ¼ 14.1; Table 1). AR patients had a mean overall PS score of 86.3 (SD ¼ 14.4), and HR patients had a mean overall PS score of 82.0 (SD ¼ 18.3). Patients in both risk groups were considered to be in the low average range. No statistically significant differences between the 2 groups were evident. Visual matching and overall PS standard scores were significantly related to age of the patient at diagnosis. The younger the child at diagnosis, the lower the visual matching score at 36 months after diagnosis (r ¼ 0.36, P , .05). The overall PS score also showed a significant relationship to age (r ¼ 0.34, P , .05). There was no significant relationship between age at diagnosis and decision speed subtest scores (r ¼ 0.22, NS). Because evaluation of PS was done 3 years after diagnosis, age of the patient at the time of evaluation showed the same relationships to outcome scores, for both strength and significance. Controls.—Healthy control patients were found to be in the average range on all 3 measures of cognitive processing: overall PS (Mean ¼ 107.4, SD ¼ 11.5), visual matching (Mean ¼ 101.4, SD ¼ 10.1), and decision speed (Mean ¼ 112.6, SD ¼ 13.8). These scores were all significantly higher than those of the patients (P , .001; Table 1).

As expected, there were no significant relationships between age of the subject at evaluation and overall PS, visual matching, or decision speed standard scores in the healthy control group (r ¼ 0.18, r ¼ 0.11, r ¼ 0.20 respectively; NS).

White Matter Integrity With use of the TBSS-based analyses as described above, a simple group-wise Student’s t test of FA values between patients and control subjects was completed. The voxels in which control subjects were found to have greater FA than that of patients were of interest. FA throughout a large majority of the fiber tracts was significantly greater in control subjects than in patients (Fig. 1, top). The second TBSS analysis evaluated voxels in which PS was significantly associated with FA, in both patients and control subjects. The multiple regression model demonstrated that overall PS was associated with widespread and numerous fiber tracts throughout the brain (Fig. 1, middle). The third TBSS analysis identified voxels that were significantly different between patients and control subjects in their association with overall PS score (Fig. 1, bottom). FA from these identified regions were categorized into 9 fiber bundles based on anatomical and functional associations,31 corpus callosum, cerebral peduncle, internal capsule, corona radiate, post thalamic radiation, sagital stratum, external capsule, cingulum, and superior longitudinal fasciculus. In each of the fiber bundles, patients had significantly lower FA, compared with control subjects (Table 2). There was no significant difference in FA between AR and HR patients in any of the bundles.

Age at Diagnosis In the patient group, FA in each of the 9 bundles was significantly related to age at diagnosis. As age of the patient increased, FA also increased. Given these findings, examination of the relationship between FA in each bundle and speed of information processing was completed controlling for age. Of the 9 bundles originally identified by the TBSS methodology, 3 bundles were found to remain significantly related to overall PS in the patient group after controlling for age at diagnosis: corpus callosum, post thalamic radiation, and external capsule.

Table 1. Speed of processing for 40 patients compared with 40 matched healthy control subjects with use of paired samples t tests (Population Mean ¼ 100, SD ¼ 15) Patients Mean (SD)

Controls Mean (SD)

t-value

P value (2-tailed)

Overall processing speed

85.62 (15.02)

107.45 (11.35)

26.79

,.001

Decision speed

92.55 (14.13)

112.65 (13.84)

26.14

,.001

Visual matching

82.22 (14.92)

101.45 (10.13)

26.28

,.001

1188

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

Fig. 1. Fractional anisotropy (FA) skeleton mask (green). Top: Voxels where FA is significantly higher (P , .01) in controls when compared to patients (red). Middle: Voxels where FA is significantly associated with overall processing speed (red). Bottom: Voxels that are significantly different between patients and controls in their association with processing speed (red).

Table 2. Mean fractional anisotropy (FA) within white matter fiber bundles associated with speed of processing and significantly different between 40 patients and 40 matched healthy control subjects Fiber Bundle

Patients Mean (SD)

Controls Mean (SD)

t value

Corpus Callosum Cerebral peduncle

0.7257 (0.047) 0.7280 (0.039)

0.7624 (0.032) 0.7712 (0.036)

24.709 (**) 26.416 (**)

Internal capsule

0.6565 (0.033)

0.6897 (0.025)

25.902 (**)

Corona radiata Post thalamic radiation

0.5403 (0.040) 0.6035 (0.044)

0.5647 (0.027) 0.6354 (0.031)

23.797 (**) 23.829 (**)

Sagittal stratum

0.5734 (0.048)

0.6118 (0.038)

24.462 (**)

External capsule Cingulum

0.5254 (0.046) 0.5580 (0.058)

0.5575 (0.033) 0.5927 (0.051)

24.079 (**) 23.591 (*)

Superior longitudinal fasciculus

0.5516 (0.034)

0.5770 (0.029)

25.236 (**)

*Significance (2-tailed) P , .01. **Significance (2-tailed) P , .001.

Results for analyses of fiber bundle FA, visual matching, and decision speed are presented in Table 3. After thalamic radiation, FA was significantly related to decision speed (r ¼ 0.34, P , .05), whereas that of the external capsule was significantly related to visual matching (r ¼ 0.42, P , .01). The corpus callosum FA was found to be significantly related to both visual matching (r ¼ 0.39, P , .05) and decision speed (r ¼ 0.35, P , .05). FA in 3 areas of the corpus callosum was examined in relation to processing outcome: genu, body,

and splenium. The genu FA was significantly related to visual matching (r ¼ 0.35, P ¼ .026) and showed a trend toward a significant relation with overall PS (r ¼ 0.31, P ¼ .05). The FA in the body of the corpus callosum was significantly related to all 3 measures of PS, visual matching (r ¼ 0.37, P ¼ .017), decision speed (r ¼ 0.32, P ¼ .044), and overall PS (r ¼ 0.38, P ¼ .016). The splenium FA showed a significant relation to decision speed (r ¼ 0.37, P ¼ .019) and overall PS (r ¼ 0.37, P ¼ .018).

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

1189

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

Table 3. Correlations of Mean white matter fractional anisotropy (FA) with measures of information processing speed within the patient group (n ¼ 40) controlling for age of the patient at evaluation Visual matching (P value)

Decision speed (P value)

Overall processing speed (P value)

Corpus callosum

0.39 (0.015)

0.35 (0.031)

0.41 (0.010)

Genu

0 0.35 (0.026)

0.21 (NS)

0.31 (0.050)

Body Splenium

0 0.37 0.017 0 0.30 (NS)

0.32 (0.044) 0.37 (0.019)

0.38 (0.016) 0.37 (0.018)

Cerebral peduncle

0.22 (NS)

0.23 (NS)

0.26 (NS)

Internal capsule Corona radiata

0.22 (NS) 0.22 (NS)

0.15 (NS) 0.21 (NS)

0.21 (NS) 0.24 (NS)

Fiber bundle

Post thalamic radiation

0.26 (NS)

0.34 (0.033)

0.33 (0.042)

Sagittal stratum External capsule

0.26 (NS) 0.42 (0.007)

0.20 (NS) 0.23 (NS)

0.26 (NS) 0.37 (0.022)

Cingulum

0.29 (NS)

0.26 (NS)

0.30 (NS)

Superior longitudinal fasciculus

0.18 (NS)

0.11 (NS)

0.17 (NS)

Fig. 2. Proposed conceptual model of the relationship between age at the time of diagnosis, fractional anisotropy (FA) of the corpus callosum, and overall processing speed. R 2 values indicate the amount of variance in the outcome variable accounted for by the predictor variable. The model suggests that impact of age at diagnosis on overall processing speed may be more directly explained by variations in corpus callosum FA.

Proposed Conceptual Model Our results suggest that a younger age at time of diagnosis is associated with increased risk for slowed PS and reduced WM FA in the corpus callosum. Even after controlling for age-related variance, relationships between the FA of the corpus callosum and each of the PS measures remained significant. Therefore, we explored a mediational model to clarify the relationship between age at diagnosis and speed of processing. We hypothesized that age at diagnosis determines corpus callosum FA, which in turn influences overall PS (Fig. 2). With use of independent R 2 values based on Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients, age at diagnosis accounted for 24.2% of the variance in corpus callosum FA (P ¼ .001). Corpus callosum FA accounted for 24.8% of the variance in overall PS (P ¼ .001). Age at diagnosis accounted for 11.7% of the variance in PS (P ¼ .03). After controlling for the effects of corpus callosum FA, however, age at diagnosis only accounted for 1.6% of the variance in PS and was

1190

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

no longer significantly related. This model suggests that the impact of age at diagnosis on overall PS may be more directly explained by variations in corpus callosum FA.

Discussion Results of the current study are relevant in determining a potential mechanism underlying the ability to process information efficiently. Evaluation of cognitive processing and related FA clearly reveals significant differences between patients treated for a posterior fossa tumor and healthy, carefully matched control subjects. Outcomes demonstrate that patients are at risk for significant deficits in PS as early as 36 months after diagnosis. Compared with healthy matched peers, patients had significantly slower overall PS, visual matching, and decision speed. The younger the patient was at time of diagnosis, the slower their PS outcome scores. Age of the patient serves as a proxy variable for processes

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

of maturation in the WM tracts throughout the brain, and therefore, the two are highly correlated.32,33 TBSS-based analyses of WM FA found significant age-related variance in both patients and control subjects. Patients were found to have significantly lower FA, compared with that of control subjects throughout widespread WM fiber tracts (Fig. 1, top), indicating pervasive disruption to patients’ WM integrity. The TBSS methodology allowed comparison of FA in each voxel to determine WM tracts that significantly differed between patients and their matched control subjects in their association with overall PS. Even after controlling for age-related variance in FA, commissural fibers within genu, body, and splenium of the corpus callosum were found to be associated with overall PS. Further investigation resulted in a proposed conceptual model of this relationship. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that PS performance in this patient group is mediated by the integrity of WM tracts located throughout the corpus callosum. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that WM integrity and associated efficiency in signal conduction intervenes the ability to process information in a timely manner. The positive relationship found between speed of processing and corpus callosum FA in the present study coincides with previous examinations of these factors in children who have suffered a traumatic brain injury.23,34 Slowed visual matching has also been significantly associated with lower FA of the corpus callosum among children with multiple sclerosis,35 and in a group of healthy children, visual matching was dependent on the WM integrity in the frontal-parietal region, mediating age-related changes in this core cognitive skill.33 Reduced FA in the splenium has also been reported in cohorts of children with deficits of attention36 who were also found to have significantly decreased functional connectivity in the posterior cingulate cortex and lateral prefrontal cortex. An examination of cognitive function in a small group of young childhood cancer survivors, including 6 patients treated for medulloblastoma and 11 treated for leukemia, found PS to be significantly related to WM FA in the body and splenium of the corpus callosum.25 However, Aukema et al. did not find age-related increases in FA and, therefore, did not correct for age in their analyses, nor did they test for mediational relationships. In healthy children, overall corpus callosum volume continues to increase throughout adolescence, largely driven by growth in the mid-regions and the splenium.32,37,38 This growth is thought to be attributable to myelination of the interhemispheric axons that compose the corpus callosum. Although significant gains in corpus callosum volume are expected for healthy children, those treated for medulloblastoma experience significant decreases in volume over time after treatment.21 Insult of cranial spinal irradiation to the corpus callosum may result in ≥1 processes, including injury to pre-existing myelination,

disruption of ongoing myelination, ischemia, and infarction.18 As the largest WM tract in the brain, the corpus callosum is easily identified and less susceptible to error in image processing. Together with the advent of DTI, precise mapping of corpus callosum projections in vivo is possible. Studies of cortical projection topography have found a significant number of connections through the corpus callosum to both the primary and extrastriate areas of the visual cortex (Broadman areas 17, 18, and 19) and the posterior areas of the parietal lobe (Broadman areas 5, 7, 23, 29, 30, and 31), encompassing the posterior cingulate.39 Broadman areas 17, 18, and 19 are vital to processing visual information. The PS tasks used in the present study rely heavily on the input of visually derived information. The patients were asked to visually scan rows of small diagrams and either circle the 2 matching objects or circle 2 objects that are most similar in meaning. Therefore, the integrity of the connections that flow through the corpus callosum would be critical to successfully performing these tasks. Limitations to the present study include the use of cross-sectional methodology. Investigation of FA in the corpus callosum over time would allow for definitive conclusions with regard to change in FA from the onset of treatment. Both volumetric and diffusion imaging would be advantageous in determining whether macrostructural changes (volume) relate to microstructural changes (FA). This may help to determine the most cost-effective and efficient manner in determining a suitable biomarker for decreases in speed of processing experienced by this group of children. Future DTI studies using longitudinal methods may hold prognostic value in determining the course of information processing deficits and understanding the vulnerability of this population of children, especially for those who receive a diagnosis at a young age.

Conclusion Treatment for a posterior fossa tumor is associated with slowed visual matching, semantic, and overall PS, compared with healthy matched peers. These deficits in function are associated with widespread reduction in the integrity of multiple WM tracts and are evident as early as 36 months after diagnosis. These results emphasize the need for neuroprotective strategies to preserve the structure and function of an increasing number of long-term survivors of pediatric posterior fossa tumors. Future research should incorporate examination of the role that WM integrity plays in mediating change in cognitive performance. Conflict of interest statement. None declared.

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

1191

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

Funding This work was supported by Cancer Center Support grants P30CA21765 and R01HD49888 from the

National Cancer Institute, the Noyes Brain Tumor Foundation, Musicians Against Childhood Cancer, and the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities.

References 1.

Gottardo NG, Gajjar A. Current therapy for medulloblastoma. Curr

2.

Ries LAG, Melbert D, Krapcho M, et al. (eds) SEER Cancer Statistics

infancy to late adulthood. Archives of Neurology. 1994;51:874 –887.

Review, 1975 – 2005, http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2005/, based

18. Leung LHT, Ooi GC, Kwong DL, et al. White matter diffusion anisotro-

on November 2007 SEER data submission, posted to the SEER web

py after chemo-irradiation: a statistical parametric mapping study and histogram analysis. NeuroImage. 2004;21:261 –268.

site. Bethesda, MD, National Cancer Institute, 2008. 3.

17. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, et al. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from

Treat Options Neurol. 2006;8:319– 334.

Gajjar A, Chintagumpala M, Ashley D, et al. Risk-adapted craniospinal

19. Reddick WE, Mulhern RK, Elkin TD, et al. A hybrid neural network anal-

radiotherapy followed by high-dose chemotherapy and stem-cell rescue

ysis of subtle brain volume differences in children surviving brain

in

children

with

newly

diagnosed

medulloblastoma

(St

Jude

Medulloblastoma-96): long-term results from a prospective, multicentre

tumors. Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 1998;16:413 –421. 20. Mulhern RK, Reddick WE, Palmer SL, et al. Neurocognitive deficits in

trial. Lancet Oncol. 2006;7:813 –820.

medulloblastoma survivors and white matter loss. Ann Neurol.

4.

Moore BD, 3rd. Neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood

1999;46:834– 841.

5.

Mulhern RK, Palmer SL. Neurocognitive late effects in pediatric cancer.

cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30:51–63.

Ness KK, Gurney JG, Zeltzer LK, et al. The impact of limitations in

22. Nagy Z, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Maturation of white matter is as-

physical, executive, and emotional function on health-related quality

sociated with the development of cognitive functions during childhood.

of life among adult survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008; 89:128 – 136. 7.

9.

J of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:1227 –1233. 23. Wilde EA, Chu Z, Bigler ED, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in the corpus callosum in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J

Schultz KA, Ness KK, Whitton J, et al. Behavioral and social out-

Neurotrauma. 2006;23:1412 –1426.

comes in adolescent survivors of childhood cancer: a report from

24. Levin HS, Wilde EA, Chu Z, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in relation to

the childhood cancer survivor study. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25:

cognitive and functional outcome of traumatic brain injury in children. J

3649 – 3656. 8.

volume among pediatric patients with medulloblastoma: longitudinal MR imaging study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2002;23:1088 –1094.

Curr Probl Cancer. 2003;27:177 –197. 6.

21. Palmer SL, Reddick WE, Glass JO, et al. Decline in corpus callosum

Head Trauma Rehabil. 2008;23:197 –208.

Mabbott DJ, Noseworthy MD, Bouffet E, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging

25. Aukema EJ, Caan MW, Oudhuis N, et al. White matter fractional an-

of white matter after cranial radiation in children for medulloblastoma:

isotropy correlates with speed of processing and motor speed in

correlation with IQ. Neuro Oncol. 2006;8:244 –252.

young childhood cancer survivors. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys.

Palmer SL. Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: a review and proposed conceptual model. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14:203 –210.

2009;74:837– 843. 26. Woodcock R, McGraw K, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson Third Edition, Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2001.

10. Briere M, Scott JG, McNall-Knapp RY, et al. Cognitive outcome in pe-

27. Alexander AL, Tsuruda JS, Parker DL. Elimination of eddy current arti-

diatric brain tumor survivors: delayed attention deficit at long term

facts in diffusion-weighted echo-planar images: the use of bipolar gra-

follow-up. Pediatric Blood Cancer. 2008;50:337 –340.

dients. Magn Reson Med. 1997;38:1016 –1021.

11. Fry AF, Hale S. Processing speed, working memory and fluid intelli-

28. Reese TG, Heid O, Weisskoff RM, et al. Reduction of eddy-

gence: evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science.

current-induced distortion in diffusion MRI using a twice-refocused

1996;7:237 –241.

spin echo. Magn Reson Med. 2003;49:177– 182.

12. Palmer SL, Goloubeva O, Reddick WE, et al. Patterns of intellectual de-

29. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional

velopment among survivors of pediatric medulloblastoma: a longitudi-

neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;

nal analysis. J Clin Oncol. 2001;19:2302 –2308.

15:1 –25.

13. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Merchant TE, et al. Neurocognitive conse-

30. Mori S, Oishi K, Jiang H, et al. Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on

quences of risk-adapted therapy for childhood medulloblastoma.

diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template. Neuroimage. 2008;

J Clin Oncol. 2005;23:5511 –5519.

40:570 –582.

14. Reddick WE, Russell JM, Glass JO, et al. Subtle white matter volume dif-

31. Yu HJ, Christodoulou C, Bhise V, et al. Multiple white matter tract ab-

ferences in children treated for medulloblastoma with conventional or

normalities underlie cognitive impairment in RRMS. Neuroimage.

reduced dose craniospinal irradiation. Magn Reson Imaging. 2000;

2012;59:3713 –3722.

18:787 – 793.

32. Barnea-Goraly N, Menon V, Eckert M, et al. White matter development

15. Reddick WE, Glass JO, Palmer SL, et al. Atypical white matter volume development in children following craniospinal irradiation. Neuro Oncol. 2005;7:12 –19. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2000;21:1099 –1109.

NEURO-ONCOLOGY

imaging study. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1848 –1854. 33. Mabbott DJ, Noseworthy M, Bouffet E, et al. White matter growth as a

16. Barkovitch AJ. Concepts of myelin and myelination in neuroradiology.

1192

during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor



SEPTEMBER 2012

mechanism of cognitive development in children. Neuroimage. 2006;33:936– 946.

Palmer et al.: White matter integrity and processing speed

34. Wu TC, Wilde EA, Bigler ED, et al. Longitudinal changes in the corpus

37. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, et al. Development of the human

callosum following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurosci.

corpus callosum during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI

2010;32:361– 373. 35. Bethune A, Tipu V, Sled JG, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging and cognitive speed in children with multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci. 2011;309: 68–74. 36. Qiu MG, Ye Z, Li QY, et al. Changes of brain structure and function in ADHD children. Brain Topogr. 2011;24:243 –252.

study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1999;23:571– 588. 38. Luders E, Thompson PM, Toga AW. The development of the corpus callosum in the healthy human brain. J Neurosci. 2010;30:10985 –10990. 39. Putnam MC, Steven MS, Doron KW, et al. Cortical projection topography of the human splenium: hemispheric asymmetry and individual differences. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;22:1662 –1669.

NEURO-ONCOLOGY



SEPTEMBER 2012

1193